2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing? Journal of Biotechnology and Biomedicine
From Jeremy Hammond: I am pleased to inform you that a paper I wrote with Dr. Brian Hooker and Dr. Jeet Varia from CHD has now been published in the Journal of Biotechnology and Biomedicine! We debunk the claim that studies have proven that “Vaccines Do Not Cause Autism”, focusing specifically on the 2019 Hviid et al. MMR-autism study out of Denmark that purported to show no increased risk even among “genetically susceptible children”.
I hope you find it useful for your own efforts and will help to get the word out about this new resource people can use for rebuttal whenever they are confronted with the big lie.
ABSTRACT
The controversy surrounding measles, mumps, and rubella (MMR) vaccination and autism has been ongoing for over 30 years. It is rooted in the parent-led grassroots movements of the 1990s; and a case-series clinical study in 1998 by Wakefield et al. This controversy cascaded through numerous observational studies and US Institute of Medicine reports, culminating in 2019 with a population-based observational study by Hviid et al. This study was hailed at the time by the US media and medical establishment as conclusive proof that the MMR vaccine does not increase the risk of autism, even among “genetically susceptible children”. However, as detailed in this critical review, Hviid et al. did not faithfully intend or interpret the data to test this hypothesis and, therefore, cannot possibly have falsified it. We elucidate methodological flaws, discrepancies, irreproducibility, and conflicts of interest for Hviid et al. In addition, the conclusion from Hviid et al. cannot be generalized to the CDC childhood vaccination schedule. All these salient features have remained oblivious to so many regulators, mainstream media, and professional associations in the USA. This reveals the need for more communication about the limitations of available evidence to facilitate informed consent for the childhood vaccination schedule.
Review Article Volume 8 • Issue 2118
Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?
Jeremy R Hammond1* , Jeet Varia PhD2 , Brian Hooker PhD2*
Affiliation:
1 Independent researcher, USA
2 Children Health Defense, 852 Franklin Ave., Suite 511, Franklin Lakes, NJ 0741, USA
*Corresponding author:
- Jeremy R Hammond, Independent researcher, USA.
- Brian Hooker, Children Health Defense, 852 Franklin Ave., Suite 511, Franklin Lakes, NJ 0741, USA.
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al.
2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Received: March 17, 2025
Accepted: March 25, 2025
Published: May 07, 2025
Abstract
The controversy surrounding measles, mumps, and rubella (MMR) vaccination and autism has been ongoing for over 30 years. It is rooted in the parent-led grassroots movements of the 1990s; and a case-series
clinical study in 1998 by Wakefield et al. This controversy cascaded
through numerous observational studies and US Institute of Medicine
reports, culminating in 2019 with a population-based observational study
by Hviid et al. This study was hailed at the time by the US media and
medical establishment as conclusive proof that the MMR vaccine does
not increase the risk of autism, even among “genetically susceptible
children”. However, as detailed in this critical review, Hviid et al. did not
faithfully intend or interpret the data to test this hypothesis and, therefore,
cannot possibly have falsified it. We elucidate methodological flaws,
discrepancies, irreproducibility, and conflicts of interest for Hviid et al. In
addition, the conclusion from Hviid et al. cannot be generalized to the CDC
childhood vaccination schedule. All these salient features have remained
oblivious to so many regulators, mainstream media, and professional
associations in the USA. This reveals the need for more communication
about the limitations of available evidence to facilitate informed consent
for the childhood vaccination schedule.
Keywords: MMR vaccination; Autism spectrum disorder; Observational
studies; Genetic susceptibility; Conflicts of interest
Introduction
In the 21st century, serious illnesses from measles, mumps, and rubella
(MMR) in the USA are all relatively rare [1-4], all representing mild, short-
lived and treatable infectious diseases. Complications are most common
in children with comorbidities or generally suffering from poor sanitation,
inadequate waste disposal systems and water supply, poverty, and deprivation.
Indeed, the CDC declared the elimination of endemic measles in 2000 [5]
and rubella in 2004 [6]. This was confirmed by Papania et al., with reported
incidence below 1 case per 1,000,000 for measles since 2001 and 1 case per
10,000,000 for rubella since 2004 [7]. Although mumps remain endemic in the
USA, Tappe et al. reported 4.54 cases per 100,000 persons in 2019 and 0.67
per 100,000 persons in 2023 [8]. However, in 2006, several mumps outbreaks
in the USA (≈2.2 per 100,000) [9] and Canada (≈75.6 per 100,000 for
adolescents) [10], were reported in highly vaccinated populations [11] [12].
Illness from MMR is of pale significance to the unprecedented rise of chronic
and autoimmune disorders in the pediatric population. For example, estimates
in the USA indicate 1 in 36 children are diagnosed with autism spectrum
disorder (ASD) [13], 1 in 10 with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD) [14], 1 in 12 with asthma [15], 1 in 4 with a food allergy [16], and
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2119
1 in 5 with one or more chronic diseases [17]. Vaccines have
been hailed as one of the greatest medical advances of the past
160 years [18]. Indeed, MMR vaccination has been declared
the protagonist for the demise of MMR in the Western world.
However, as discussed by Guyer et al. [19] “Nearly 90% of
the decline in infectious disease mortality among US children
occurred before 1940 when few antibiotics or vaccines were
available.” Hence, “vaccination does not account for the
impressive declines in mortality seen in the first half of the
century.” In addition, evidence detailing the durability and
longanimity of natural immunity vs. vaccination is resounding
[20-22]. This also has further implications for infants whose
early protection derives from passive maternal immunity via
the placenta or postnatally via breast milk [23-25]. Moreover,
natural infections experienced during childhood, such as
measles and mumps, can encourage normal immune system
development, with reports of protecting effects against
Parkinson’s disease [26], chronic lymphoid leukemia [27],
cardiovascular disease [28], follicular B-cell non-Hodgkin
lymphoma [29] and allergies [30,31]. Evidence that surviving
measles infection confers beneficial effects beyond lifelong
protection from measles disease further indicates the need for
policymakers to consider natural immunity as an opportunity
cost of vaccination. It also reinforces the necessity for long-
term studies comparing a broad range of health outcomes,
including all-cause mortality, between fully vaccinated and
completely unvaccinated children [32].
In 2013, a Cochrane collaboration research review by
Demicheli et al. [33], reported significant evidence of adverse
events from MMR vaccines. Although the scientists did not
present statistical confirmation of the existence or a reliable
relationship between MMR vaccinations and ASD diagnoses,
they did report finding that “problematic internal validity in
some included studies and the biases present in the studies
(selection, performance, attrition, detection, and reporting)
influenced our confidence in their findings”; that “The design
and reporting of safety outcomes in MMR vaccine studies,
both pre- and post-marketing, are largely inadequate”; and
that “The evidence of adverse events following immunization
with the MMR vaccine cannot be separated from its role
in preventing the target diseases.” Indeed, as discussed by
Miller [34], MMR vaccination has many documented safety
deficits that counteract well-publicized benefits. For example,
MMR vaccination has been attributed to the increased risk of
emergency hospitalizations, seizures, and thrombocytopenia,
a serious bleeding disorder.
The US government and mainstream media routinely
propagate the claim that scientific studies have conclusively
demonstrated that vaccines cannot cause ASD. The Centers
for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) authoritatively
declares on its website that “vaccines do not cause autism”
[35]. To support its bold proclamation, the agency cites
several reports commissioned by the Institute of Medicine
(IoM), now the National Institute of Medicine: in 2004
(“Immunization Safety Review”) [36], 2011 (“Effects of
Vaccines: Evidence and Causality”) [37], and 2013 (“The
Childhood Immunization shedule and Safety”) [38]. The
2004 report concluded “that the evidence favors rejection
of a causal relationship between MMR vaccine and autism.”
(p. 7). However, the same IoM report acknowledges “the
possibility that MMR could contribute to autism in a small
number of children because the epidemiological studies
lacked sufficient precision to assess rare occurrences; it was
possible, for example, that epidemiological studies would not
detect a relationship between autism and MMR vaccination
in a subset of the population with a genetic predisposition
to autism. The biological models for an association between
MMR and autism were not established but not disproved” (p
4). Although the 2004 IoM subtitled the report “Vaccines and
Autism” and has erroneously been construed to indemnify
all vaccines from the autism epidemic, the committee only
ruled on a single vaccine and a single ingredient, the MMR
vaccine or the use of thimerosal, respectively. Concerning the
MMR vaccine, the. 2004 IoM acknowledged “the possibility
that MMR could contribute to autism in a small number of
children” (p 4) and that the types of observational studies
that had been done “would not detect a relationship between
autism and MMR vaccination in a subset of the population
with a genetic predisposition to autism” (p 4). The IoM 2011
report further concluded as the 2004 IoM report that the
evidence favors a rejection of a causal relationship; however,
this conclusion was based principally on four observational
studies, each failing to consider the possibility of “genetically
susceptible subpopulations.” The 2013 IoM report was an
update to earlier reports addressing the safety of the entire
infant/child immunization schedule. The committee found
that “Studies designed to examine the long-term effects of
the cumulative number of vaccines or other aspects of the
immunization schedule have not been conducted” (p 5).
Consequently, the IoM reviews fail to support the claim for
which the CDC cites them.
Wakefield et al. 1998
Media, institutional, and public hysteria surrounding
the MMR vaccine can be traced back to a case series
clinical study of 12 children with regressive developmental
disorder, including nine with ASD, by Wakefield et al. in
1998 [39]. In 2004, the same year that the IoM issued its
report concluding that “no convincing evidence exists for
the casual MMR autism”, the Lancet published “retraction
of interpretation” for Wakefield et al. [40], with a full
retraction in 2010. This culminated in what can essentially
be described as the “professional castration” of lead author
Andrew Wakefield and coauthor John Walker-Smith in 2010
by UK journalist Brian Deer in the British Medical Journal
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2120
[41-43]. In this case series clinical report, Wakefield et
- describe a pattern of inflammatory disorders of the gut
(colitis and ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia) in children
with autism in association with MMR vaccination. Of note,
the hypotheses generated by the study, although perceived at
the time as almost idiosyncratic, were not new. Work here
followed previous speculation from 1994 by Wakefield and
coworkers of a causal relationship between the measles virus
[44-46] and MMR vaccination [47] with Crohn's disease. In
the conclusions of their 1998 publication, Wakefield et al.
explicitly stated, “We did not prove an association between
measles, mumps, and rubella vaccine and the syndrome
described. We have identified chronic enterocolitis in
children that may be related to neuropsychiatric dysfunction.
In most cases, the onset of symptoms was after measles,
mumps, and rubella immunization.” Although they do
suggest, “Further investigations are needed to examine
this syndrome and its possible relation to this vaccine.”
To this day, a causal association between ASD and MMR
vaccination is still heavily contested. However, nearly 26
years after its publication, as speculated by Wakefield and
coworkers in the early '00s [48,49], substantial evidence
provides not only a correlational but causal relationship
between gut inflammation, gut pathology, and the gut-brain
axis in the etiology, pathogenesis, and pathophysiology of
ASD [50]. Indeed, in 2010, an expert panel of the American
Association of Pediatrics (AAP) “an organization of
60,000 pediatricians” [51], strongly recommended further
investigation into the role of gastrointestinal abnormalities in
children with ASD [52].
The controversy over MMR and ASD can and should
be further traced to a social movement of parents who
mobilized around concern over the MMR vaccination,
dating from the early 1990s in the UK. Parents reported
developmentally normal infancy with sudden regression
around the middle of their second or fourth year [53].
Children become withdrawn, with symptoms later diagnosed
as part of the autistic spectrum, along with severe and
painful bowel problems. Reflecting on the timing, many
parents came to link developmental regression and autistic
symptoms to MMR vaccination is defined here as “abuse
aimed at making victims question their sanity as well as the
veracity and legitimacy of their perspectives and feelings”
[54]. The plight of the patients was further compounded
by their experiences of limited governmental and societal
recognition of their accounts. Medical gaslighting is not new
to MMR and ASD, with growing recognition that the modern
allopathic “diagnose-protocol-prescription-paradigm” has
created a wider gap between the practitioner and patient
[55]. Indeed, medical gaslighting appears to be becoming
more common, especially for those illnesses reported to
be vaccine-induced [56] or contested [57]. The gaslighting
and shared experiences and understanding of parents who
reported injury and developmental delays in association with
MMR vaccination ultimately led to a “parental-Wakefield
alliance.” Their media reportage became a serious concern
to scientists and policymakers embroiled in public health
and vaccination in the UK, Europe, and the USA. Leach
frames this as a contrasting individual or paternal vs. public
commitment [58]. Parents were primarily concerned about
what they saw as the vaccine-damaged health of their
children. Government policymakers and their supportive
scientific networks had institutional commitments to the
continued integrity of a vaccination program with its public
obligations and population-level imperatives. Some mention
must also be given to the interests of the pharmaceutical
industry [59], i.e., “blockbuster” monopoly and profit margin
for their shareholders, maximized through millions spent on
marketing and lobbying for their products [60].
Scientific speculations, controversy, criminal allegations,
and nuances [61,62] concerning the case of Wakefield et
al., are certainly not the spotlight of this study. However,
the vitriol, hysteria, and polarization of perspectives
highlighted above give the backdrop, along with a cascade
of observational studies investigating the association between
MMR vaccination and ASD [63,64]. The debate turns, in part,
on the significance attributed to epidemiological as opposed
to clinical evidence and on the status attributed to parents’
observations and paternal instincts [65], culminating in 2019
with the publication of Hviid et al.
Hviid et al. 2019
In 2019, an observational study by Hviid et al. [66]
was published that was hailed by the mainstream media,
especially in the USA, as demonstrating irrevocably that the
MMR vaccine cannot cause autism, even among “genetically
susceptible children.” The study was published in the journal
Annals of Internal Medicine on March 5, 2019, and titled
“Measles, Mumps, Rubella Vaccination and Autism: A
Nationwide Cohort Study.” It was authored by Anders Hviid,
Jørgen Vinsløv Hansen, Morten Frisch, and Mads Melbye.
The AAP claimed, “Another study has confirmed children
who receive measles, mumps and rubella vaccine (MMR) are
not at increased risk of autism”, and that the findings “also
held for vaccinated children with a sibling who had been
diagnosed with autism. Among girls, the risk of autism was
lower in those who were vaccinated” [67]. Here are some
further illustrative examples of how the US mainstream
media reported on the study by Hviid et al.:
- A CNN headline declared, “MMR vaccine does not cause
autism, another study confirms.” Emphasizing that the
“biggest contribution of the study was the inclusion of
children at risk of autism”, CNN reported that vaccines
do “not increase the risk of autism and does not trigger
autism in children who are at risk” [68].
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2121
- A headline from National Public Radio (NPR) similarly
declared, “A Large Study Provides More Evidence That
MMR Vaccines Don’t Cause Autism.” This article quoted
lead author Anders Hviid conclusively stating that “MMR
does not cause autism.” The study, according to NPR,
“found no increased risk among subgroups of children
who might be unusually susceptible to autism, such as
those with a brother or sister with the disorder” [69].
- The headline of a LiveScience article about the study
stated, “Confirmed: No Link Between Autism and Measles
Vaccine, even for ‘At Risk’ Kids” [70].
- A headline in the New York Times trumpeted, “One More
Time, With Big Data: Measles Vaccine Doesn’t Cause
Autism” [71].
- “Another Massive Study Finds Measles Vaccine Doesn’t
Cause Autism”, said the headline of a Healthline article
that quoted coauthor Mads Melbye saying, “It’s time to
bury the hypothesis that MMR causes autism” [72].
- MedicalNewsToday reported, “MMR vaccine does not
cause autism, even in those most at risk” [73].
- “Study Again Confirms No Link Between MMR Vaccine
and Autism”, read the headline of a Psychiatry Advisor
article claiming that the study showed the vaccine “does
not trigger autism in children who are susceptible to the
disorder” [74].
- The New Yorker magazine stated, “The science on this
point is settled, to the extent that any science ever is, in
the pursuit of proving a negative” [75].
The media characterized the study as rejecting the
hypothesis of a causal association between MMR vaccine
and autism in “susceptible children.” However, as discussed
shortly, Hviid et al. excluded children who had any one
of several genetic conditions specifically because those
conditions are associated with an increased risk of autism.
Not one of those media reports relayed this salient fact to
readers. Nor, for that matter, was there even the slightest
critical examination by the media or the AAP of the study’s
methodology, findings, and conclusions. Contrary to what
we’ve been told by mainstream media, as discussed in
this critical commentary, the study of Hviid et al. cannot
conclusively demonstrate that the MMR vaccine does not
cause ASD in “susceptible children.” Moreover, it certainly
also does not falsify the hypothesis that the MMR vaccine
or vaccines administered according to the CDC’s schedule
can contribute to the development of ASD in susceptible
children. Indeed, there are substantial nuances within the
study by Hviid et al., a critical examination of which reveals
that it was not faithfully applied to test this hypothesis and
therefore cannot possibly have falsified it.
Study Overview
Aims
A preceding retrospective cohort study (children born
in Denmark 1991 – 1998) by Madsen and colleagues,
including Hviid and Melbye, was published in 2002 in The
New England Journal of Medicine (NEJM) [76], and titled,
“A population-based study of measles, mumps, and rubella
vaccination and autism.” Using analogous methodologies
and data sources, Madsen et al. 2002 concluded as Hviid
et al. that “This study provides strong evidence against the
hypothesis that MMR vaccination causes autism.” Hviid et
- referred to this previous study and stated, “In this study,
we aimed to evaluate the association again in a more recent
and nonoverlapping cohort of Danish children that has
greater statistical power owing to more children, more cases,
and longer follow-up”, and “To evaluate whether the MMR
vaccine increases the risk for autism in children, subgroups of
children, or periods after vaccination.” The authors note the
criticism that the earlier study “did not address the concern
that MMR vaccination could trigger autism in specific groups
of presumably susceptible children.” They claimed that their
new study “addresses this concern in detail” by evaluating
“the risk for autism after MMR vaccination in subgroups of
children defined according to environmental and familial
autism risk factors.”
Study Design, Methodology and Demographic, and
Conclusions
The authors analyzed data for 663,236 children born in
Denmark to Danish-born mothers from January 1, 1999,
through December 31, 2010. Of these children, 5,775 were
excluded, resulting in a cohort of 657,461 children. The
observation period was from age one until August 31, 2013,
so the earliest-born children had reached the age of fourteen
by the end of follow-up, whereas the latest-born were still
as young as two years. The total number of children who
were followed until the end of the study was 650,943, and
among these children, 6,517 (1%) had received a diagnosis
of autism as of the follow-up end date. The average age of
autism diagnosis for their study population was 7.22 years for
children born in Denmark from January 1994 – 1999. Parner
et al. by contrast, reported in 2008 that the average age of
autism diagnosis in Denmark was 5 – 6 years, observing a
decrease in the age of diagnosis over the study period [77].
Overall, the Hviid et al. study population was about 95%
“vaccinated”, with an average vaccination age of 1.34 years
(≈ 16 months). Among children with autism, 5,992 (92%),
were “vaccinated” and 525 (8%) were “unvaccinated.”
Hviid et al. summarize their methodology as follows;
“Survival analysis of the time to autism diagnosis with Cox
proportional hazards regression was used to estimate hazard
ratios of autism according to MMR vaccination status, with
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2122
adjustment for age, birth year, sex, other childhood vaccines,
sibling history of autism, and autism risk factors (based on a
disease risk score).”
The Cox regression model has been primarily applied
by researchers for time-to-event analysis and allows one
to estimate the hazard ratio (HR) of a given endpoint
associated with a specific risk factor. Analysis by survival
or time-to-event is frequently used in epidemiological and
clinical studies [78]. As discussed by Anderson et al., time-
to-event data typically feature challenges related to, among
other things, censored observations and changes over time
in the absolute or relative risks, as well as in the values of
the predictors. In the context of “rare events” like autism,
such approaches can suffer from erratic behavior [79]. A
fundamental assumption underlying the application of the
Cox model is proportional hazards; in other words, the effects
of different variables on survival are constant over time and
additive over a particular scale [80]. The chosen methodology
also involved comparing the cumulative incidence of autism
for the “vaccinated” and “unvaccinated” cohorts, calculated
as the number of new events or cases of a disease divided by
the total number of individuals in the population at risk for
a specific time interval. Thereby, a child remained “at risk”
of developing autism until they received an autism diagnosis
or were otherwise “censored” from the study, meaning that
they ceased to be included in the population of children under
observation and hence ceased contributing to “person-years”
at risk.
Children in the cohort contributed person-time to follow-
up from 1 year of age to the end of the study on 31 August
2013, until a first diagnosis of autism, or censorship. So, for
example, a child born in 1999 who was uncensored from
the study until its end without having received an autism
diagnosis would have contributed 14 years of “person-time”
(or “person-years”) at risk, whereas a child born the same
year who received an autism diagnosis in 2004 would have
contributed five person-years at risk. The incidence rate
among the study population was 129.7 cases of diagnosed
autism per 100,000 person-years. Children were excluded due
to diagnosis of any of several genetic disorders or conditions
or censored due to death, emigration, or disappearance.
The key finding from the main analysis of the study was
that “vaccinated” children were not at a higher risk of autism
than “unvaccinated” children. As stated in the abstract,
“Comparing MMR-vaccinated with MMR-unvaccinated
children yielded a fully adjusted autism hazard ratio of 0.93
(95% CI, 0.85 to 1.02).” Figure 3 from Hviid et al. further
summarizes HRs, confidence intervals, and p-values of
correlation. Except for the reduced association of autism
and the MMR vaccination for females (HR = 0.79, 95%
CI = 0.64 – 0.97) all p-values were > 0.05, indicating no
significant association. On this basis, the authors made bold
conclusions that “The study strongly supports that MMR
vaccination does not increase the risk for autism, does not
trigger autism in susceptible children, and is not associated
with clustering of autism cases after vaccination. It adds to
previous studies through significant additional statistical
power and by addressing hypotheses of susceptible subgroups
and clustering of cases.” However, there are numerous
reasons why their findings can and should not support these
bold conclusions, including major study flaws, numerous
discrepancies, and unexplained analysis; salient features of
which remained oblivious to regulators, associations, and
mainstream media in the USA.
Study Design Flaws
Misleading definition of “genetic susceptibility”,
exclusion of children with high susceptibility and
inadequate sample size
One of the criticisms of the studies cited by the CDC
to support its claim that “vaccines do not cause autism” is
that they do not consider the possibility of “susceptible
subpopulations.” Hviid et al. acknowledge this and state,
“Specific definitions of susceptible subgroups have been
lacking.” The authors reference and follow the lead of Jain et
- [81] in defining “genetic susceptibility” merely as having
“a sibling history of autism” at the time of study entrance.
Therefore, if a child had an autistic sibling, but the sibling
was not diagnosed until after the child had entered the
study, then the child would have been misclassified as not
“genetically susceptible.” Likewise, if a child had a genetic
or environmental susceptibility but had no siblings, the
child would have been wrongly classified. 49% of the study
population was defined as having no “genetic susceptibility”
simply by being an only child, as there were 319,936 children
with no siblings out of a study population of 657,461.
The central dogma that autism is a highly heritable
genetic disease is under debate. In 2011, Hallmayer et al.
[82], in the largest twin study to date, reported moderate
genetic heritability of 37-38% [83]. Later in 2014, using an
epidemiological sample from Sweden, Gaugler et al. [84]
concluded that autism’s genetic architecture has a narrow-
sense heritability of ≈52.4%, with most due to the common
variation and rare de novo mutations. Considering current
evidence of moderate autism genetic heritability, if a child
had a genetic susceptibility and one or more siblings, but no
siblings sharing the genetic trait or environmental trigger,
the child would be wrongly classified as not “genetically
susceptible.”
Moreover, and counterintuitive to the stated aims of the
study, while touting their study as being designed to “address
the concern that MMR vaccination could trigger autism in
specific groups of presumably susceptible children”, Hviid et
- excluded 620 children who received a diagnosis during
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2123
the first year of life of any of the following genetic disorders:
neurofibromatosis, tuberous sclerosis, Angelman syndrome,
Fragile X syndrome, Prader-Willi syndrome, Down syndrome,
and DiGeorge syndrome. All these disorders demonstrate a
higher rate of comorbidity with autism [85-91]. While they
did not explain their rationale for these exclusionary criteria,
their working assumption was presumably as follows: if these
children were later diagnosed with autism, then it was due to
their underlying condition and not vaccination. However, that
syllogism is a non sequitur fallacy; the conclusion doesn’t
follow from the premise. In effect, Hviid et al. treated all these
conditions as competing hypotheses. One would contend
they should have treated them as potential risk factors or
indicators of epigenetic susceptibilities that might predispose
these children to vaccine injury manifesting as symptoms of
autism. Therefore, by excluding those children, the authors
acted directly contrary to their stated purpose to investigate
if “vaccination could trigger autism in specific groups of
presumably susceptible children.”
As we have seen, the media touted this study as “large”,
quoting a study population of 657,461. However, what the
media consistently failed to point out is that only a small
number met the authors’ definition of being “genetically
susceptible”, with only 838 (0.13%) children meeting the
criterion of having a sibling with autism. Following on, the
reported HR for “siblings with autism” indicated an HR of
2.69 for autism (95% CI 0.58 – 12.63) among those who
received the MMR vaccine compared to those who didn’t.
Although this correlation was not statistically significant,
one can speculate the result may have been significant if
the authors had not excluded the 620 children with genetic
disorders. We will never know since the authors have refused
to release their underlying data to other scientists to be able to
reproduce the authors’ findings.
Apart from genetic factors, Hviid et al. developed an
“autism risk score” based on several “environmental autism
risk factors”, but these were limited to “maternal age,
paternal age, smoking during pregnancy, method of delivery,
preterm birth, 5-minute Apgar score, low birth weight,
and head circumference.” Although a low 5-minute Apgar
score, low birth weight, and large head circumference may
be indicative of a developing autism phenotype, it would
be incorrect to label them as “risk factors” in the etiology
or pathogenesis of ASD. In addition, apart from “smoking
during pregnancy”, there is no consideration for assessment
of the risk from xenobiotic environmental insults. This
includes a long legacy of scientific literature spanning many
decades implicating exposure to pharmaceuticals, industrial
chemicals, and toxic and heavy metals in the etiology of
ASD [92]. Future studies should apply a more rigorous risk
assessment of exposure to environmental toxins [93-95] and
other socioeconomic factors. For example, scientists could
develop a risk score for exposure to environmental toxins
based on factors including but not restricted to geographical
location. Recent work by Palmer et al. [96] provides state-of-
the-art approaches for rigorous assessment of chemical risk
factors and intolerance in children and parents of developing
autism and ADHD. This includes a complete evaluation of
symptoms, intolerances, and life impacts of chemical, food,
and drug exposures.
The second entry for the definition of the verb “lie”
in Merriam-Webster’s dictionary is “to create a false or
misleading impression” [97]. We would infer this is precisely
what Hviid et al did when they delivered the public message
that their study proved that the MMR vaccine “doesn’t cause
autism even in children who are at greater risk of autism”
or “genetically susceptible.” Indeed, Hviid et al. defined
“genetic susceptibility” and did include children who met
their definition. However, as outlined above, such an “ad
hoc” definition of “genetic susceptibility” lacks scientific
rigor and is inadequate and misleading.
Failure to control for “healthy user bias”
Jain et al.
A “healthy user bias” has been highlighted in previous
studies for vaccination uptake [98-100]. In this scenario,
parents of children who show symptoms at an early age or
who have an older MMR-vaccinated sibling with autism,
developmental delays, or other chronic disease, are more
likely to skip the MMR vaccine, thereby biasing correlations
in favor of finding no association. This “healthy user bias”
was acknowledged by Hviid et al., who reference the study of
Jain et al., [101] which “identified lower MMR uptake rates
in children with affected siblings.” Published in 2015, Jain et
- investigated autism occurrence by MMR vaccine status
among US children with older siblings with and without
autism. The conclusion drawn by Jain et al. was that “In this
large sample of privately insured children with older siblings,
receipt of the MMR vaccine was not associated with increased
risk of ASD, regardless of whether older siblings had ASD.”
Based on the lower vaccine uptake among children who were
considered at higher risk of autism due to “genetic factors”,
such conclusions would need reevaluation.
To illustrate, as detailed by Jain et al., whereas the MMR
vaccination rate for children with unaffected siblings was 84%
at age 2 years and 92% at age 5, by contrast, the vaccination
rate for children with autistic older siblings was 73% at age
2 and 86% at age 5. This would indicate that parents whose
first child is diagnosed with autism after receipt of the MMR
vaccine are less likely to get the shot for their second child
for fear that it might contribute to the development of autism
in the younger sibling. Similarly, parents who notice early
developmental delays might skip the MMR vaccine for
fear of it contributing to the development of autism. In the
authors’ own words, considering that lower relative risk
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2124
(RR) estimates were observed among children with autistic
older siblings versus children with unaffected siblings, “It is
possible, for example, that this pattern is driven by selective
parental decision-making around MMR immunization, i.e.,
parents who notice social or communication delays in their
children decide to forestall vaccination. Because as a group
children with recognized delays are likely to be at higher
risk of ASD, such selectivity could result in a tendency for
some higher-risk children to be unexposed . . . . It is also
plausible that parents of affected older siblings would be
especially attentive to developmental delays in their younger
children and decide to forestall immunization.” Thus, Jain
et al. reasonably hypothesized that families with one child
already affected by autism might be particularly concerned
about this for any younger siblings, resulting in a lower
vaccination rate among “genetically susceptible children.”
In addition, although not statistically significant, Jain et
- found a negative correlation between the rate of autism
in children with an autistic older sibling and receipt of the
MMR vaccine. Rather than indicating some protective effect
of the vaccine, we would speculate this would further indicate
confounding by “healthy user bias.” This is an inherent risk
of confounding in all observational studies, which needs to be
accounted for and controlled for.
Hviid et al.
We know that Hviid et al. were aware of “health user
selection bias” because they cited Jain et al. and acknowledged
their finding of lower vaccine uptake among “susceptible
children.” Yet they failed to account for it. Indeed, Hviid
et al. affirmed that children who had siblings with autism
had 7.32 times greater HR (95% CI 5.29 - 10.12) of autism
relative to children who had siblings without autism. They
also acknowledged the finding of Jain et al. that children with
an autistic older sibling were less likely to receive the MMR
vaccine. However, based on their analysis of the Danish study
population, they claimed to have observed a vaccination
rate of 95.19% and “no appreciable differences in vaccine
uptake according to . . . . autism history in siblings.” On
closer examination of the data, alternative interpretations
are needed. For girls, the authors leaped baselessly to the
conclusion that the vaccine is protective, asserting that MMR
vaccination “reduced the risk for autism in girls.” We would
conjecture that the overall negative (HR = 0.79, 95% CI 0.64
– 0.97) association could instead be due to girls at “higher
risk” being less likely to receive the vaccine. The study’s table
of population characteristics shows that 838 of the children
in the study population had a sibling with autism, among
whom 759 (90.6%) were MMR-vaccinated and 79 (9.4%)
were not. Thus, whereas in the general study population
only 4.8% were “unvaccinated”, the proportion who were
unvaccinated among “genetically susceptible” children was
nearly double that. Figure 3 from Hviid et al. further shows
that among these 838 “genetically susceptible children”, 37
(4.4%, or 1 in 23) were diagnosed with autism. As discussed
earlier, the HR shown for this cohort indicates a 2.69 times
greater risk of autism among “vaccinated” children compared
to “unvaccinated”. Among the 37 children diagnosed with
autism, 32 were “vaccinated”, and 5 were “unvaccinated.”
Therefore, 4.2% of the susceptible vaccinated children had
an autism diagnosis compared to 6.3% of the susceptible
unvaccinated children, indicating a possible pooling of
children at “higher risk” into the “unvaccinated” group.
To approach the question from yet another angle, 759
of the 625,842 “vaccinated” children had an autistic sibling
compared to 79 of the 31,619 “unvaccinated” children.
Therefore, 0.12% of “vaccinated” compared to 0.25% of
“unvaccinated” children were “genetically susceptible.” The
“unvaccinated” were thus twice as likely to be “genetically
susceptible” according to the author’s definition. In addition,
the table shows that 319,936 children in the study had no
siblings, among whom 4.2% were “unvaccinated”, and
331,994 had siblings without autism, among whom 5.3%
were “unvaccinated.” These proportions contrast with the
9.4% of children with autistic siblings being “unvaccinated.”
Children considered “genetically susceptible” were thus 1.8
times more likely than children with non-autistic siblings and
2.3 times more likely than single children to remain MMR-
unvaccinated. Looking again at environmental risk factors
for autism, Table 1 of the study shows that 3.97% of “very-
low risk”, 4.35% of “low risk”, 5.44% of “moderate risk”,
and 6.79% of “high-risk” children remained “unvaccinated.”
Therefore, “high-risk” children were 1.25 times more
likely than “moderate risk”, 1.56 times more likely than
“low-risk” and 1.71 times more likely than “very low-risk”
children to remain MMR-unvaccinated. This would again
indicate potential confounding of “healthy user bias” with
environmental and genetic risk factors.
Hviid et al. describe their study as “by far the largest
single study to date” and state that it “allows us to conclude
from one study that even minute increases in autism risk after
MMR vaccination are unlikely, assuming unbiased results.”
If their findings instead reflect the same “healthy user bias”
identified by Jain et al., then the study by Hviid et al. does not
allow us to draw such a conclusion. Strikingly, Hviid et al.
acknowledge this limitation: “If the onset of symptoms results
in avoidance of vaccination”, they admitted, bias in favor of
no association “is possible.” Should they have said, “likely?”
Failure to consider all vaccines routinely
recommended for children in Denmark
The study of Hviid et al. focused only on the effect of the
MMR vaccine on autism rates and not the complete Danish
childhood vaccine schedule. In a secondary analysis, they
also considered other routinely administered vaccines as
covariables. From this secondary analysis, they concluded
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2125
that “MMR vaccination did not increase the risk for autism in
children characterized by other early childhood vaccinations.
. . .” However, this analysis was limited to include only “other
childhood vaccinations administered in the first year of life”,
with no consideration of vaccinations given to children after
twelve months other than the first MMR dose typically given
at fifteen months. Other vaccines considered by Hviid et al.
included five of the six vaccines recommended by Danish
health authorities for routine use in infants under one year
of age [102,103]. Hviid et al. described the “mainstays” of
the early Danish vaccine schedule as consisting of “MMR
and diphtheria, tetanus, acellular pertussis, inactivated
polio, and Haemophilus influenzae type b (DTaP-IPV/
Hib) combination.” In Denmark, the combination of DTaP,
IPV, and Hib vaccines has been recommended for infants,
each with a three-dose course. However, Hviid et al. failed
to consider that Danish authorities had since October 2007
additionally recommended three doses of the pneumococcal
conjugate vaccine (PCV) during the first year of life [104].
The introduced formulation was the 7-valent PCV7, replaced
with the 13-valent PCV13 starting in April 2010 [105]. An
additional fourth “booster” dose of DTaP-IPV combination
vaccine is also recommended at age 5 [106]. A four-dose
course of hepatitis B (HepB) vaccine was recommended in
2005 for children born to mothers who are a carrier at birth
and then 1, 2, and 3 months of age [107], which Hviid et al.
further failed to consider. The authors also failed to consider
the human papillomavirus vaccine (HPV) [108,109].
According to the World Health Organization (WHO)
[110], “The primary target group in most of the countries
recommending HPV vaccination is young adolescent girls,
aged 9-14.” In Denmark, Merck’s [111] quadrivalent HPV
vaccine Gardasil was introduced in October 2008 as a
catch-up program targeting 12-year-old girls, with routine
vaccination for girls aged 12 years starting in January 2009.
The study’s follow-up period was from January 1, 2000,
through August 31, 2013, and girls of this initial birth cohort
would have reached the age of thirteen or fourteen. While
girls born in subsequent cohorts would have been too young
to receive the HPV vaccine (unless administered earlier than
the age of 12), girls in this 1999 – 2001 cohort may have
received the HPV vaccine starting in 2011.
Thus, while this is not a flaw in the study per se, the choice
by Hviid et al. to narrow their focus fails to meaningfully
address parents’ concerns about the long-term effects on
health outcomes of the complete and extended Danish
vaccine schedule [112]. However, even if they had done such
a study, its findings would not have been generalizable to
the US childhood population since Denmark has a different
schedule than that recommended in the USA.
Failure to account for MMR formulation change
According to Hviid et al., the MMR vaccine used
in Denmark from 2000 through 2007 contained the
Schwarz strain of measles virus, which would have been
GlaxoSmithKline’s (GSK’s) “Priorix” vaccine, and a
different formulation was used from 2008 through 2013 that
contained the Enders’ Edmonton strain, which would have
been Merck’s “MMR-II.” This would indicate that children
using the Merck formulation were much too young to receive
an autism diagnosis as the oldest they would be at the time
of study is 6 years of age or younger. On further evaluation,
however, the information provided by Hviid et al. is incorrect;
they mistakenly reversed the order in which MMR vaccines
were used during those periods. According to a 2018 study on
the use of the MMR vaccine in Denmark by Sørup et al., [113]
until 2008, the Danish vaccination program used Merck’s
MMR-II, which was marketed in Europe as “Virivac” and
contained the Enders Edmonston B strain of measles virus
[114-116]. From mid-October 2008, “Virivac” was replaced
by GSK’s “Priorix”, which contained the Schwarz strain of
measles virus [117]. Since mid-June 2013, a new version
of Merck’s MMR-II has been used, which is manufactured
by Sanofi Pasteur and marketed as “MMRvaxPro”, and
which likewise contains the Edmonston strain of measles
virus [118-121]. Coauthor Christine Stabell Benn (personal
communication, August 19, 2024) [122] corresponded with
lead author Signe Sørup to confirm that the information in
their paper was correct. GSK’s Priorix was used from 2008
until 2013, not Merck’s MMR-II, as mistakenly reported by
Hviid et al. [123] (supplementary material, Appendix 1).
Following on, the average age of autism diagnosis for their
study population was 7.22 years, and the typical age of first
MMR vaccination in Denmark is 15 months. Since the study’s
follow-up period ended on August 31, 2013, children who
received “Priorix” would have been under 5 years of age and
thus, on average, too young to receive an autism diagnosis
[124]. Again, this could bias the study in favor of finding no
association between the vaccine administered to the 2008 –
2010 birth cohort and the risk of autism.
Children too young for autism diagnosis
Hviid et al., report the average age of the sample as 8.64
years with a standard deviation (SD) of 3.48 years. The
average age of autism diagnosis is reported as 7.22 years, with
an SD of 2.86 years. If the age of diagnosis follows a normal
distribution, 34.2% of the sample (z = -0.408) would be too
young to get an autism diagnosis. This could account for as
many as 3,387 additional cases not included in the analysis,
which would further bias the outcomes to favor acceptance of
the null hypothesis and no association between MMR vaccine
and autism.
Failure to consider a change of recommended age
for 2nd MMR dose
Hviid et al. did not consider the second dose of the MMR
vaccine in their primary analysis. In a secondary analysis,
they reported “no evidence of a dose-response.” However,
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2126
they failed to account for a change in the recommended age
at which the second dose was administered. The first dose of
MMR vaccine is recommended for children in Denmark at
15 months, followed by a second dose at the age of 4 years.
However, before April 2008, the second dose was routinely
administered at age 12 [125]. Therefore, children in the birth
cohorts of 1999 – 2001 and 2002 – 2004 would not have
received the second dose until years after the average age (7.22
years) of an autism diagnosis for the overall study population.
Receiving the second dose of the MMR vaccine earlier in
childhood development rather than in early adolescence may
be associated with an increased risk of autism. The inclusion
in the secondary analysis of those cohorts of older children
who did not get both doses during early childhood would
again bias the results erroneously in favor of acceptance of
the null hypothesis.
Failure to consider maternal vaccination
Maternal vaccination is another factor that Hviid et al.
failed to account for in their study. It is recognized within
the scientific community that maternal inflammation is
associated with the development of autism in the offspring
[126]. Vaccines intended to produce an immune response
involving inflammation mechanisms could infiltrate the
placenta, compromise fetal development, and increase
the risk of ASD in offspring [127-129]. In alignment with
recommendations by the WHO [130], the Danish Health
Authority, since 2010, has recommended seasonal trivalent
inactivated split influenza virus vaccination for pregnant
women with selected high-risk chronic diseases in any
trimester; and vaccination is additionally recommended for
all pregnant women in the second and third trimesters [131].
Mølgaard-Nielsen et al. report up to 10% vaccine uptake by
Danish women between 2010 – 2016. This means children
born in the last cohort and reaching the age of 3 years by the
end of the follow-up period may have been born to mothers
vaccinated during pregnancy. Future studies should account
for prenatal risk factors, including vaccination and the use of
other pharmaceuticals during pregnancy.
Exclusion of immigrants
Hviid et al. included only children “born to Danish-born
mothers from 1 January 1999 through 31 December 2010”
and registered in the Danish Medical Birth Registry, with the
exclusion of 1,498 children. Asylum seekers to Europe may
come from war-torn countries where health systems have
broken down. There is evidence that asylum-seeking children
have low coverage of childhood vaccinations in their home
countries, as well as high uptake of immunizations in host
countries [132] [133]. Therefore, immigrant children might
receive multiple vaccines, doses, and boosters at once or
spaced closer together to “catch up” on ones they may have
missed in their home country. This might place immigrant
children at higher risk of vaccine injury and developmental
disorders such as autism. In addition, children of non-Danish
ethnicity may have a higher risk of autism due to one or
more epigenetic traits. Their exclusion could further bias the
study’s findings.
Potential misclassification of study subjects
As the authors acknowledge, “A limitation of our study
is that we used the date of first diagnosis of autism, which
is probably delayed compared with the age at onset of
symptoms.” They then suggest this might bias their findings
in favor of an association between MMR vaccination and
autism by citing a hypothetical example in which “symptoms
precede vaccination and diagnosis occurs after vaccination”,
resulting in “misclassification of autism cases as vaccinated.”
While they focus on the hypothetical scenario of bias favoring
an association, they do not account for the misclassification of
“vaccinated” children as “unvaccinated.” A study published in
2017 by Holt et al. [134], using data from the Danish National
Health Service Register, addressed known concerns about the
reliability of vaccination coverage data. To that end, the study
authors compared MMR vaccination coverage according to
medical records from general practitioners with that reported
by the national registration database. Researchers report that
the national database showed significantly lower vaccine
coverage than medical records. Among the practices included
in the study, the national database showed vaccine coverage
of 86%, whereas the medical records showed coverage of
94%. The study authors state, “More than half of the children
who were unvaccinated according to the register-based data
(55%) had been vaccinated according to the medical records.”
Vaccinated children being misclassified as “unvaccinated” in
the study by Hviid et al. would, of course, bias their findings
in favor of the null hypothesis and no association.
Discrepancies in Autism Rate in the Study
Group vs. Danish Population
In Figure 1 of their study, Hviid et al. report 6,517
children with autism out of a population of 650,943,
equating to 1%. This includes only subjects followed until
the end of the study. If one takes account of the 657,461
children initially included, an autism prevalence of 0.99%
can be calculated. The prevalence of autism in Denmark
in 2016, according to a study published by Schendel and
Thorsteinsson [135], was 1.65%. Given a study population of
657,461 children, of whom 650,943 were followed until the
end of the follow-up period, at a prevalence rate of 1.65%,
we should expect there to be between 10,741 and 10,848
children with autism, whereas, in the study population, there
were only 6,517. This would indicate an under-ascertainment
of between 4,224 and 4,331. However, although the Hviid et
- study was published in 2019, the observation period for
the study population ended on August 31, 2013. Therefore,
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2127
the prevalence of autism for that year would make the most
valid comparison. According to the study of Schendel and
Thorsteinsson among children aged 10 years, the prevalence
of autism was 1.16% by 2010 (representing the birth cohort
of 2000 – 2001), 1.33% by 2012 (birth cohort 2002 – 2003),
1.44% by 2014 (birth cohort 2004 – 2005), and 1.65%
by 2016 (birth cohort 2006 – 2007). Given an estimated
prevalence by 2012 of 1.33%, we would expect the Hviid et
- study population to include 8,658 to 8,744 children with
autism, indicating that approximately 2,141 to 2,227 autistic
children were missing from the study. This suggests either
that Schendel and Thorsteinsson's estimated prevalence of
autism was grossly inaccurate or that the population under
study by Hviid et al. was not representative of the childhood
population of Denmark. The latter explanation is more likely
as the study of Schendel and Thorsteinsson, unlike Hviid et
al., was designed to estimate prevalence, and its findings were
consistent with CDC data for the US childhood population,
with an observed increase in the prevalence of autism for
each birth cohort [136]. By contrast, when broken down by
the age of each birth cohort, the data presented by Hviid et
- show a decreasing prevalence of autism. Figure 3 from
Hviid et al. summarizes the total number of children with
autism for each birth cohort. This enables one to calculate the
prevalence of diagnosis based on age for each birth cohort;
namely, 1.71% for the 1999 – 2001 cohort, 1.28% for 2002
– 2004, 0.74% for 2005 – 2007, and 0.20% for 2008 – 2010.
These discrepancies would indicate methodological flaws in
the study of Hviid et al. that render their study population
non-representative. The authors do not acknowledge these
discrepancies, much less provide any explanation.
Irreproducible Findings
Reproducibility is an essential aspect of the scientific
method [137]. It is crucial to advancing scientific knowledge
because it ensures that research findings are reliable and
not due to error, chance, or bias. Without reproducibility,
scientific claims remain unverified and are therefore of
questionable reliability. While the data Hviid et al. present
show a decreasing rate of autism from one birth cohort to
the next, they contradictorily state in their paper that being in
the later-born 2008 – 2010 cohort conferred the “highest risk
for autism.” However, on inspection of Table 3 in the SI of
Hviid et. al, using the 1999 – 2001 cohort for reference, they
report an HR of 1.18 for 2002 – 2004, 1.31 for 2005 – 2007,
and 1.34 for 2008 – 2010. So, children born in 2009 - 2010
were 1.34 times more likely to be diagnosed with autism than
those born in 1999 – 2001, and so on. While this increasing
risk is what we would expect to find, as shown, it directly
contradicts the data shown in their paper. This puzzling
discrepancy was noticed by statistician Elizabeth Clarkson
(personal communication, April 4, 2019), who contacted the
Annals of Internal Medicine staff and the study’s lead author,
Anders Hviid, to inquire about this self-contradiction and to
request their raw data (supplementary material, Appendix 2)
[138]. In reply to Clarkson’s email inquiry, Hviid confirmed
that the trend shown by their HR could not be reproduced
from the data they presented in the main paper. However, he
said, they could not release their raw data because they were
“prohibited from sharing these data by Danish law.” Clarkson
then wrote the Annals editors to formally request the authors’
dataset, pointing out that “the results of this sophisticated
regression model used for the results reported in Table 3 of
the supplemental material is in direct contradiction to the
crude associations computed from the data published in the
paper itself.” In reply to Elizabeth Clarkson, the journal staff
instructed her to direct her request to the study’s authors. Since
there is a major self-contradiction between the data reported
and their calculated HRs, serious concerns must be raised
about their true scientific viability and irreproducibility.
Unexplained Risk of Autism Incidence for Boys
and Girls with Genetic Susceptibility
In the abstract, Hviid et al. included the caveat that
“no increased risk for autism after MMR vaccination was
consistently observed in subgroups of children defined
according to sibling history of autism.” One interpretation of
the adverb “consistently” would logically imply an increased
risk was observed in at least one such subgroup of children.
Indeed, for boys who had an autistic sibling, Figure 4 in the
SI of Hviid et al. illustrates the cumulative incidence, which
represents the male children who met the author’s criterion
for having a “genetic susceptibility” to autism. From about
age seven onward, the higher cumulative incidence of autism
was among the children who received the MMR vaccine.
This increased risk of autism among vaccinated boys was not
statistically significant, which may be an artifact of a small
subset of boys considered in this analysis.
The same figure illustrates the cumulative incidence
of autism among girls with an autistic sibling. From
approximately age 4 - 11, among girls with “genetic
susceptibility”, a greater cumulative incidence for those who
received the MMR vaccine is displayed. However, between
the ages of 11 and 12, there is a leap in the cumulative
incidence for MMR-unvaccinated girls from approximately
1% to approximately 9%, resulting in a greater incidence of
autism among the unvaccinated. Since the study’s follow-up
period ended in 2013, the maximum age of 14 on this graph
can only represent girls born in 1999. Comparably, only
children born in or before 2002 could have reached the age
of 11 before the study’s end. One can only speculate on the
cause of the sudden increase in the cumulative incidence of
autism at about age 11, which would be relevant to the 1999
– 2001 and 2002 – 2004 birth cohorts. The authors do not
discuss this sudden increase in cumulative incidence, which
is certainly a curiosity for which an explanation is warranted
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2128
but lacking.
Non-Generalizability to the US Childhood
Population
Taking account of the endorsement of Hviid et al. by
the USA´s media, regulators, and professional medical
organizations and their claims that the vaccine-autism
hypothesis had been falsified, the question arises if the
conclusions presented here can be generalized to the US
population. While Danish health authorities recommend
the HepB vaccine for infants (considered) at risk, the
CDC recommends a three-dose regimen of this aluminum-
containing vaccine for all infants starting from the first day
of birth. Similar to Denmark, during their first year of life,
children in the US typically receive three doses each of DTaP,
IPV, Hib, and pneumococcal conjugate vaccine (PCV15 or
PCV20); but starting at the age of 6 months, American children
also receive two or three doses of rotavirus vaccine (RV1
or RV5, respectively) and an inactivated influenza vaccine,
multi-dose formulations of which contain the preservative
thimerosal [139]. The rotavirus vaccine is not recommended
in Denmark, and whereas Danish authorities recommend
flu shots only for children aged 2 to 6 years and adults aged
65 or older, the CDC recommends annual flu shots, multi-
dose vials of which also contain thimerosal, a mercury-based
preservative, starting in infancy and continuing throughout
an individual’s lifetime. Whereas Danish children receive a
booster dose of DTaP and IPV at the age of 5 years, American
children receive a fourth dose of IPV at 4 years, and for DTaP,
a fourth dose at the age of 15 months, a fifth dose at the age
of 4 years, and a booster dose of the adolescent and adult
formulation Tdap at the age of 11 years. While in Denmark
the first dose of MMR is typically given at 15 months,
it is recommended earlier in the US, at 12 months, with a
second dose in both countries at 4 years of age. The varicella
or “chickenpox” vaccine is not on Denmark’s childhood
schedule but is recommended by the CDC at the age of 1 year
and a second dose at age 4. The hepatitis A vaccine is also
not on Denmark’s schedule, while the CDC recommends it
in a two-dose series spaced six months apart starting at the
age of 1 year. The meningococcal vaccine is another shot not
recommended for routine use in Denmark, whereas the CDC
recommends it at age 11, with a second dose at age 16. While
the HPV vaccine is recommended in Denmark for children
aged 12 years, the CDC recommends its two-dose regimen
starting at age 11 while okaying its administration for
children as young as 9. Additionally, the CDC recommends
that pregnant women receive the aluminum-containing Tdap
vaccine, the potentially thimerosal-containing influenza
vaccine, and the respiratory syncytial virus vaccine [140].
In summary, the CDC recommends upwards of 70 vaccine
doses for 17 diseases, with a whopping 29 doses (20 or more
injections) by a neonate’s first birthday. At a two-month
“well-childcare visit”, an infant may receive as many as six
vaccines for eight pathogens. In comparison, the Danish
schedule consists of twelve shots for six pathogens, with
only four vaccines by their first birthday (three doses each of
DTaP, IPV, Hib, and PCV13). Once again, so many opinion
leaders, regulators, media, and professional associations in
the USA were oblivious to these salient differences.
Conflicts of Interest
The stakes in the ASD debate are high. Over half a century,
there has been a dramatic increase in ASD rates [141].
Identifying causative factors for ASD is already a challenging
task for the scientific community, demanding the highest
standards of openness and transparency. Any departure from
these standards represents a disservice to all. The financial
stakes in vaccine research are also high. The global biologics
market was US$511.04 billion in 2024 and is expected to reach
around US$1,374.51 billion by 2033 [142]. The approval and
subsequent commercialization of gene therapy candidates are
expected to drive growth in the biologics market. The vaccine
market worldwide was valued at US$81.06 billion by 2023
and is anticipated to reach around US$152.45 billion by 2033
[143]. In the U.S., the COVID-19 vaccine market transitioned
to a commercial phase following the depletion of the federal
government’s purchased stock. The global mumps vaccine
market was valued at US$2 billion in 2021 and is projected to
reach US$3.5 billion in 2031 [144].
When conflicts of interest influence research, the
resulting scientific debate on safety and efficiency, etc.,
can be confounded by misleading information. Indeed, to
ensure scientific quality, manuscripts authored by CDC staff
are required to undergo an internal review and approval
process known as clearance. As part of the domain of ethical
standards, “free from conflicts of interests” is explicitly stated
[145]. Kern et al., [146] summarize past and current examples
of research conflict of interest and outside influences for
tobacco, lead, methylmercury, atrazine, bisphenol A, and
olestra. The CDC receives millions of dollars in industry
gifts and funding, including substantial support from the
pharmaceutical industry [147,148]. Miller and Goldman
[149] and Hooker [150,151] provide firsthand details of how
the CDC suppressed and disallowed deleterious vaccine
data from being published and engaged in other acts of
questionable scientific integrity. Nissen [152] discusses the
dependence of professional medical associations on industry
funding [153].
Physicians and the public rely on journals as unbiased
and independent sources of information and to provide
leadership to improve trust in medicine and the medical
literature. Yet financial conflicts of interest have repeatedly
eroded the medical profession's and journals’ credibility
[154]. During the past decade, two former editors-in-chief
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2129
of the NEJM, Marcia Angell and Arnold Relman, have
spoken out about the excessive power of the pharmaceutical
industry over medical research, hospitals, and doctors. In a
letter to the New York Times on December 28, 2004 [155],
they pointed out that in the previous year, one drug company
had spent 28 percent of its revenues (more than $6 billion)
on marketing and administrative expenses. They concluded,
“The medical profession should break its dependence on the
pharmaceutical industry and educate its own.” In an article in
the New York Review of Books on January 15, 2009, Angell
wrote, “It is simply no longer possible to believe much of the
clinical research that is published, or to rely on the judgment
of trusted physicians or authoritative medical guidelines”
[156].
Hviid et al.
Hviid and his three coauthors (Hansen, Frisch, and
Melbye), at the time of the study’s publication, were affiliated
with the Statens Serum Institut (SSI), which develops
vaccines and is “responsible for the purchase and supply
of vaccines to the Danish national vaccination programs”
[157]. Like the CDC, the SSI is a government agency and
research institute; its purpose is to “ensure preparedness
against infectious diseases and biological threats as well as
control of congenital disorders.” For vaccine research, the
SSI is “devoted to vaccines against tuberculosis, chlamydia,
HIV and novel adjuvants to direct and potentiate the immune
responses.” Upon the study’s publication, the SSI issued a
press release proclaiming that it “once again invalidates
the claim that the MMR vaccine increases children’s risk of
developing autism” [158].
Financial support was provided by the Novo Nordisk
Foundation [159,160] and the Danish Ministry of Health
[161]. The Novo Nordisk Foundation is a charitable
foundation that issues funding grants for scientific research
while owning the holding company Novo Holdings A/S
[162], the majority voting shareholder in the Danish
pharmaceutical corporation Novo Nordisk [163]. Novo
Nordisk is a large multinational pharmaceutical company
in Denmark with a market capitalization greater than
US$497 billion [164]. According to their annual report,
they anticipated an effective 2019 tax rate of 20-22%; the
government of Denmark receives significant tax revenue
from Novo Nordisk. Both the Danish Ministry of Health and
Novo Nordisk have a vested interest in a study that might
influence the demand for the MMR vaccine. Also of note,
Novo Holdings A/S investments include vaccine companies
[165]. For example, in April 2019, the group invested tens
of millions of dollars into Oxford Biomedica, which was
involved in a consortium to develop and manufacture the
AstraZeneca COVID-19 vaccine [166,167]. In December
2023, the Novo Nordisk Foundation Initiative for Vaccines
and Immunity (NIVI) was announced, [168] which is a
partnership between the University of Copenhagen, and the
SSI [169,170]. The stated goal of NIVI is “to revolutionize
and accelerate vaccine development in Denmark by bridging
the gap between academic research and industry innovation.”
Simultaneously, the foundation established a limited
liability company, the Novo Nordisk Foundation Vaccine
Accelerator, to “facilitate the translational efforts of NIVI
by providing industry-level expertise in vaccine development
and conducting the early clinical testing of our vaccine
candidates” [171]. Predating its vaccine initiative, the Novo
Nordisk Foundation had funded numerous researchers in SSI
and other institutions active in vaccine-related research [172-
177]. The foundation also funded the SSI “Danish National
Biobank”, which aims to grant scientists access to data on
residents in Denmark from national health registries along
with information about biological samples [178]. The Danish
Ministry of Health and the SSI unquestionably have a stake
in preserving their own credibility and existing policies, not
unlike the United States Department of Health and Human
Services and CDC. However, as summarized by Fig. 1 and
as is the documented case with American regulatory agencies
and professional bodies [179], an inherent conflict of interest
in researching, marketing, and supplying childhood vaccines
becomes apparent, if not explicit.
Figure 1: Ceonflicts of Interest Concerning Hviid et al.
Annals of Internal Medicine
Two editors of the Annals of Internal Medicine, Jaya K.
Rao (Deputy Editor) and Catharine B. Stack (Deputy Editor
for Statistics), disclosed holding stocks in pharmaceutical
companies active in vaccine research and manufacture. This
includes Eli Lilly, Pfizer, and Johnson and Johnson. Eli Lilly
is a former manufacturer of Dr. Jonas Salk’s inactivated polio
vaccine and the developer of the mercury-based preservative
thimerosal [180].
Discussion
In 2019, the AAP and mainstream media in the USA
hailed the study by Hviid et al. as additional proof that the
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2130
MMR vaccine does not increase the risk of ASD, even among
“genetically susceptible children.” But the fact is that the study
authors excluded children with any one of several genetic
conditions placing them at higher risk, with an inadequate
definition of “genetic susceptibility.” Based on highlighted
methodological flaws, discrepancies, and conflicts of interest,
we would venture that the outcomes from Hviid et al. would
not indicate evidence of a lack of association between ASD
and MMR but, instead, researcher bias to a priori serve
the status quo. As an antidote, we would prescribe diligent
scientists working in the field to take note and learn from
Hviid et al. with a priori consideration of selection bias
and risk factors, “healthy user bias”, and data calibration
with positive and negative controls [181], which would
provide paths to much-needed rigor in observation studies;
especially when the stakes are so high, with CDC objectives
for ubiquitous vaccination in pediatric populations.
Hviid and coworkers have used similar methodologies to
assess risks of other adverse events and disorders from MMR
vaccination and other pharmaceuticals used in pregnancy or
childhood. For example, Hviid and coworkers have reported
no association between ASD and thimerosal-containing
vaccines [182]; no evidence of causality for childhood
vaccination and type 1 diabetes [183]; an increased rate of
febrile seizure following MMR vaccination deemed “small
even in high-risk children” [184]; no significant association
between maternal use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors
during pregnancy and ASD in their offspring [185]; and no
association between Ondanse (prescribed for nausea and
vomiting) and increased risk of adverse fetal outcomes [186].
We would note that in studies for febrile seizure and type I
diabetes in pediatric populations, the “genetic susceptibility”
evaluation, as for Hviid et al. 2019, was based on family
history, i.e., sibling history of adverse events or diabetes
diagnosis. Clinical and preclinical evidence of adverse events
from these pharmaceuticals is well documented [187-190].
Further critical analysis of these studies, as provided here for
Hviid et al. 2019, would be prudent, commended, and much
needed.
In a famous case, the government acknowledged that the
administration of nine vaccine doses at once to a 19-month-
old girl named Hannah Poling “significantly aggravated an
underlying mitochondrial disorder, which predisposed her to
deficits in cellular energy metabolism and manifested as a
regressive encephalopathy with features of autism spectrum
disorder” [191]. At the time, then CDC director Julie
Gerberding admitted on CNN [192], “Now, we all know that
vaccines can occasionally cause fevers in kids. So, if a child
was immunized, got a fever, had other complications from
the vaccines, and if you’re predisposed to a mitochondrial
disorder, it can certainly set off some damage. Some of the
symptoms can be symptoms that have characteristics of
autism.” Notably, in 2009 Gerberding left her government
job to work for the pharmaceutical giant Merck as president
of their vaccine division and later became responsible for
“strategic communications” as Chief Patent Officer and
Executive Vice President of the Company, Population Health
& Sustainability [193,194].
Indeed, a growing body of evidence implicates a strong
interplay between environmental insults and epigenetics in the
etiology and pathogenesis of ASD [195]. Bradstreet argued in
a presentation given to the Vaccine Safety Committee in 2004
[196] that, “meaningful epidemiological studies should test
a priori hypotheses that derive from all clues evident in the
clinical histories of affected children . . . .” We would concur
and endorse future prospective observational studies that
truly account for epigenetic and environmental risk factors
as part of the wider “ecological exposome” [197]. Although
a non-trivial task, the SSI biobank initiative provides Danish
researchers access to “25 million biological samples” [198],
which should be ample and adequate to define and address
the association between genetic susceptibility, MMR, and
ASD. In a 2014 interview with journalist Sharyl Attkisson,
the CDC’s Director of the Immunization Safety Office, Frank
DeStefano, acknowledged that “it’s a possibility” that vaccines
could trigger ASD in “genetically susceptible individuals”,
but that it is “hard to predict who those children might be”,
and trying to determine what underlying conditions put
children at greater risk of being injured by vaccines is “very
difficult to do” [199]. Indeed, researchers at the CDC have
acknowledged that no observational study “can definitively
establish or disprove the hypothesis that thimerosal exposure
increases the risk of ASDs”, which would instead require “a
large-scale prospective randomized trial” [200].
No golden standard exists to judge if an observed
association (or non-association) is genuine [201]. Placebo-
controlled randomized clinical trials (RCT), although
also prone to error [202], remain the gold standard for the
inference of causality. However, no placebo-controlled RCT
has investigated individual vaccines, let alone the overall
effects of the vaccine schedule, for long-term health outcomes
between vaccinated and unvaccinated children, including
all-cause mortality. The common consensus among FDA
regulators is that it is unethical to do “vax-unvax” clinical
studies with true saline placebos [203]. We would agree,
inasmuch as puncturing any baby’s skin over the course of a
year with at least 20 intramuscular injections of saline placebo
or multiple “biologic pharmaceuticals”, all formulated
differently, many in one sitting, as part of any clinical study,
would not only be unethical but barbaric. Moreover, this is
an exemplary example of the petitio principii fallacy and
institutional cognitive dissonance for vaccine research since
it presumes a priori that the potential benefits outweigh any
possible risks, which is precisely the proposition that should
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2131
have been determined, as also argued by Bradstreet, through
properly designed, rigorous, and robust RCTs. Despite this,
a word of caution would be prudent. We would state that no
“magic bullet” exists for testing causality, especially for rare
and/or long-term serious adverse events. Indeed, questions
of causality cannot be answered by RCTs alone because of
the inherent low power in such studies [204]. Some discourse
must also be given to the manufacturing of pharmaceutical
biologics. Whereas the chemical synthesis of a small
molecule drug may have a dozen steps that must be monitored
and controlled, the fermentation process for vaccines may
have hundreds [205]. Valiant et al. [206] and Chooi et al.
[207] provide ample examples of past and current reports of
contamination issues and vaccine recalls. Unquestionably,
it is an important risk factor for vaccine injury, as would
be the case for any pharmaceutical intended for ubiquitous
prophylaxis of pediatric populations [208].
The challenges in making inferences from the evidence
are no less great (but not particularly greater) when the
evidence is based on large observational studies, as per Hviid
et al. (or a small clinical case series, as per Wakefield et al.).
However, if one truly takes an evidence-based approach, it
would be ill-advised to be complacent about immunization
and assume its innocence based on observational studies
or even systematic reviews of the evidence [209]. As
early as 2004, Price, Jefferson, and Demicheli highlight
methodological issues arising from systematic reviews of
vaccine safety, especially for rare and/or long-term serious
adverse events [210]. Moreover, a growing body of scientists,
healthcare professionals, and citizens question the benefits of
vaccines, with evidence that child mortality and disease from
infectious diseases had already decreased significantly before
the widespread use [211-213]. Based on their observations,
one can hope that immunization will become redundant
through good sanitation, adequate waste disposal systems,
clean water, nutritious food supply, wealth, and plenty for
all children [214]. Nonetheless, the possibility remains that
we may be creating long-term damage through vaccination,
demonstrated by substantially increasing levels of serious
and chronic disease, including autoimmune conditions,
especially in populations made vulnerable by acute or chronic
anthropogenic xenobiotic insult [215, 216].
Hviid et al. provide an example of how studies examining
this question concerning the MMR vaccine could be interpreted
as having been designed to find no association through design
flaws biasing findings. Moreover, to date, no studies have
been done that were designed to test the hypothesis that
vaccinating according to the CDC’s schedule can contribute
to the development of autism, chronic diseases, and all-cause
mortality in children with epidemiological susceptibility.
Therefore, scientists and medical practitioners must always
be on their guard for evidence pointing to vaccine danger, as
should be the case for side effects from any pharmaceutical
intervention [217]. Evidence alone never speaks for itself or
conveys the truth because it always requires interpretation.
Expert opinion or population-based studies are not a
surrogate for evidence-based, first-hand experience or data;
science is not about consensus, it’s about the truth [218, 219].
Moreover, acquiring the right answers also requires asking
the right questions. Barosi and Gale illustrate the point when
they state that “Accuracy refers to getting the correct answer,
and precision to getting the same answer on repetition
regardless of whether it is the correct answer or not. A wrong
answer which is reproducible is precise but inaccurate. What
we need are accurate, precise answers.” Based on the case
presented here, for Hviid et al., nothing could be more vital
for the future of vaccine research.
Closing Remarks
Vaccine safety science has become a hazardous occupation
[220]. The sanctity of vaccines has become a religious
mantra, and anyone who questions the safety or efficacy
of government-recommended vaccines or deviates from
acceptable vaccine orthodoxy is vulnerable to suppression
and personal attacks [221]. The consequences can be severe,
with harm to reputation, hindrance of research, and even
destruction of a career. The tactics of suppression reported
by the researchers and doctors in the study of Elisha et al.
[222] refer to defamatory publications on websites, retraction
of papers that pointed to safety issues with certain vaccines,
denial of research grants, calls for dismissal, summonses to
hearings or disciplinary committees by health authorities,
suspensions of medical licenses, and self-censoring.
Kempner [223] further defines the “chilling effect” regarding
the influence of political controversy on the production of
new science. It appears that so many researchers are trapped,
restrained, and handicapped in an “institutional straitjacket”
[224] to continue their studies while employing practices
specifically designed to disguise the most controversial
aspects of their research and maintain the consensus and
status quo.
Alongside this, as highlighted by this study, the public
is poorly served by the coverage of medical science in the
press. Journalists in the medical field are often accused of
being sensational, unobjective, and speculative, with a lack
of follow-up and undisclosed conflicts of interest [225,226].
To illustrate, The New York Times health writer Aaron E.
Carroll has advocated public compliance with the CDC’s
influenza vaccine recommendations, which include the
recommendation that pregnant women get a flu shot [227].
He thus treats the observational studies that the CDC relies
on to support the claim as conclusive proof that vaccination
during pregnancy is safe for both the expectant mother and the
vulnerable developing fetus. Yet when it comes to the risks
of drinking alcohol, Carroll advises his readers, “Don’t give
too much weight to observational data” [228]. Lipworth et al.
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2132
[229] venture that ‘alternative media’, while threatening the
status quo, viability, and public dependence on mainstream
media, allows patients and clinicians to engage in media-led
open debates about health-related issues based on first-hand
experience.
The autism-MMR debate is entrenched in the many
decades of grassroots movements rooted in the 1990s. All
were motivated by the medical gaslighting of mothers and
fathers, medical indifference to their paternal instinct, and
wider social ostracization of those who show any deceit toward
the accepted vaccine orthodoxy. Over 30 years later, in the
post-COVID-19 era, stereotyping, social stigma, shunning,
condescension, and polarization of parents who choose not to
vaccinate their children [230] and the vaccine injured [231]
has only been exacerbated and intensified. We would propose
a moratorium on the stigmatization and dichotomization of
the unvaccinated, the vaccine-injured, and vaccine critics and
an end to mandates for childhood vaccines for school entry.
Health freedom, parental autonomy, and open, frank scientific
debate can only foster real advancements for true service to
our children, families, and the wider society. Indeed, as stated
by Aristotle: “Science arises from curiosity.” We would
extend this to explicitly state that it certainly cannot arise
from institutional conflicts of interest, consensus, censorship,
imposition, suppression, apathy, or gaslighting.
Funding
This research received no specific grant from funding
agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Data Availability
No data was used for the research described in the article.
Acknowledgments
Consultation, critical insights, and/or comments for the
first draft from JB Handley, James Lyons-Weiler, Stephanie
Seneff, and Elizabeth Clarkson.
Competing Interests : The author(s) declare none.
Credit Authorship Contribution Statement
Hammond created the original study conception and first
draft. Additional concept development and drafting by Varia
and Hooker. Further writing, review, and editing by Varia,
Hooker, and Hammond. All authors have read and approved
the final manuscript.
Supplementary Files Link:
https://cdn.fortunejournals.com/supply/JBB12526.pdf
References
- Perry RT, Halsey NA. The clinical significance of
measles: A review. The Journal of Infectious Diseases
189 (2004): S4-S16.
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC).
Mumps. Mumps Symptoms and Complications. (2024).
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC).
Rubella (German Measles, Three-Day Measles). (2024).
- DeStefano F, Shimabukuro TT. The MMR vaccine and
autism. Annual Review of Virology 6 (2019): 585-600.
- Mathis AD. Measles—United States, January 1, 2020–
March 28, 2024. MMWR. Morbidity and Mortality
Weekly Report (2024) 73.
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC).
Elimination of rubella and congenital rubella syndrome-
United States, 1969-2004. MMWR. Morbidity &
Mortality Weekly Report 54 (2005): 279-282.
- Papania MJ, Wallace GS, Rota PA, et al. Elimination of
endemic measles, rubella, and congenital rubella syndrome
from the Western hemisphere: The US experience. JAMA
Pediatrics 168 (2014): 148-155.
- Tappe J, Leung J, Mathis AD, et al. Characteristics of
reported mumps cases in the United States: 2018–2023.
Vaccine 42 (2024): 126143.
- Barskey AE, Glasser JW, LeBaron CW. Mumps
resurgences in the United States: A historical perspective
on unexpected elements. Vaccine 27 (2009): 6186-6195.
- De Serres G, Markowski F, Toth E, et al. Largest measles
epidemic in North America in a decade-Quebec, Canada,
2011: Contribution of susceptibility, serendipity, and
superspreading events. The Journal of Infectious Diseases
207 (2013): 990-998.
- Dayan GH, Rubin S, Plotkin S. Mumps outbreaks in
vaccinated populations: Are available mumps vaccines
effective enough to prevent outbreaks? Clinical Infectious
Diseases 47 (2008): 1458-1467.
- Poland GA, Jacobson RM. Failure to reach the goal
of measles elimination: Apparent paradox of measles
infections in immunized persons. Archives of Internal
Medicine 154 (1994): 1815-1820.
- Maenner MJ, Warren Z, Williams AR, et al. Prevalence
and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among
children aged 8 years—Autism and Developmental
Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 sites, United States,
- MMWR Surveillance Summaries 72 (2023): 1.
- Li Y, Yan X, Li Q, et al. Prevalence and trends in diagnosed
ADHD among us children and adolescents, 2017-2022.
JAMA Network Open 6 (2023): e2336872-e2336872.
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2133
- Zahran HS, Bailey CM, Damon SA, et al. Vital signs:
Asthma in children—United States, 2001–2016.
Morbidity & Mortality Weekly Report 67 (2018): 149.
- Zablotsky B, Black LI, Akinbami LJ. Diagnosed allergic
conditions in children aged 0-17 years: United States,
- NCHS Data Brief. No. 459. National Center for
Health Statistics. (2023).
- Ullah F, Kaelber DC. Using large aggregated de-identified
electronic health record data to determine the prevalence
of common chronic diseases in pediatric patients who
visited primary care clinics. Academic Pediatrics 21
(2021): 1084-1093.
- Killeen RM. Vaccines-one of the greatest medical
advances of modern times. Canadian Pharmacists Journal/
Revue des Pharmaciens du Canada, 140 (2007): S2-S2.
- Guyer B, Freedman MA, Strobino DM, et al. Annual
summary of vital statistics: Tcrends in the health of
Americans during the 20th century. Pediatrics 106 (2000):
1307-1317.
- Bianchi FP, Mascipinto S, Stefanizzi P, et al. Long-term
immunogenicity after measles vaccine vs. wild infection:
An Italian retrospective cohort study. Human Vaccines &
Immunotherapeutics 17 (2021): 2078-2084.
- Horstmann DM, Schluederberg A, Emmons JE, et al.
Persistence of vaccine-induced immune responses to
rubella: comparison with natural infection. Clinical
Infectious Diseases 7 (1985): S80-S85.
- Amanna IJ, Carlson NE, Slifka MK. Duration of humoral
immunity to common viral and vaccine antigens. New
England Journal of Medicine 357 (2007): 1903-1915.
- Papania M, Baughman AL, Lee S, et al. Increased
susceptibility to measles in infants in the United States.
Pediatrics, 104 (1999): e59-e59.
- Mandomando IM, Naniche D, Pasetti MF, et al. Measles-
specific neutralizing antibodies in rural Mozambique:
Seroprevalence and presence in breast milk. American
Journal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene 79 (2008):
787-792.
- Waaijenborg S, Hahné SJ, Mollema L, et al. Waning of
maternal antibodies against measles, mumps, rubella, and
varicella in communities with contrasting vaccination
coverage. The Journal of Infectious Diseases 208 (2013):
10-16.
- Sasco AJ, Paffenbarger JR, RS. Measles infection and
Parkinson's disease. American Journal of Epidemiology
122 (1985): 1017-1031.
- Parodi S, Crosignani P, Miligi L, et al. Childhood
infectious diseases and risk of leukaemia in an adult
population. International Journal of Cancer 133 (2013):
1892-1899.
- Kubota Y, Iso H, Tamakoshi A, et al. Association of
measles and mumps with cardiovascular disease: The
Japan Collaborative Cohort (JACC) study. Atherosclerosis
241 (2015): 682-686.
- Montella M, Dal Maso L, Crispo A, et al. Do childhood
diseases affect NHL and HL risk? A case-control study
from northern and southern Italy. Leukemia Research 30
(2006): 917-922.
- Rosenlund H, Bergström A, Alm JS, et al. Allergic
disease and atopic sensitization in children in relation to
measles vaccination and measles infection. Pediatrics 123
(2009): 771-778.
- Shaheen SO, Barker DJP, Heyes CB, et al. Measles and
atopy in Guinea-Bissau. The Lancet 347 (1996): 1792-
1796.
- Benn CS, Amenyogbe N, Björkman A, et al. Implications
of non-specific effects for testing, approving, and
regulating vaccines. Drug Safety 46 (2023): 439-448.
- Demicheli V, Rivetti A, Debalini MG, et al. Vaccines for
measles, mumps and rubella in children. Evidence‐Based
Child Health: A Cochrane Review Journal 8 (2013):
2076-2238.
- Miller NZ. Vaccines and sudden infant death: An analysis
of the VAERS database 1990–2019 and review of the
medical literature. Toxicology Reports 8 (2021): 1324-
1335.
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). CDC
vaccine safety. 2024. Autism and Vaccines. (2024).
36.Board on Health Promotion, Disease Prevention, &
Immunization Safety Review Committee. (2004).
Immunization safety review: Vaccines and autism.
Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US).
(2004).
37.Committee to Review Adverse Effects of Vaccines;
Institute of Medicine. Adverse effects of vaccines:
Evidence and causality. Clayton, E. W., Rusch, E.,
Ford, A., & Stratton, K. (Eds.). (2012). Adverse effects
of vaccines: Evidence and causality. Washington (DC):
National Academies Press (US). (2012).
- Board on Population Health, Public Health Practice,
& Committee on the Assessment of Studies of Health
Outcomes Related to the Recommended Childhood
Immunization Schedule. (2013). The childhood
immunization schedule and safety: Stakeholder concerns,
scientific evidence, and future studies. Washington (DC):
National Academies Press (US). (2013).
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2134
- Wakefield AJ, Murch SH, Anthony A, et al. RETRACTED:
Ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, non-specific colitis,
and pervasive developmental disorder in children. The
Lancet 351 (1998): 637-641.
- Murch SH, Anthony A, Casson DH, et al. Retraction of
an interpretation. The Lancet 363 (2004): 750.
- Deer B. Wakefield’s “autistic enterocolitis” under the
microscope. BMJ (2010): 340.
- Deer B. How the vaccine crisis was meant to make money.
BMJ (2011): 342.
- Deer B. How the case against the MMR vaccine was
fixed. BMJ (2011): 342.
- Ekbom A, Adami HO, Wakefield A, et al. Perinatal
measles infection and subsequent Crohn's disease. The
Lancet 344 (1994): 508-510.
- Wakefield AJ, Ekbom A, Dhillon AP, et al. Crohn's
disease: Pathogenesis and persistent measles virus
infection. Gastroenterology 108 (1995): 911-916.
- Ekbom A, Daszak P, Kraaz W, et al. Crohn's disease after
in-utero measles virus exposure. The Lancet 348 (1996):
515-517.
- Thompson NP, Pounder RE, Wakefield AJ, et al. Is
measles vaccination a risk factor for inflammatory bowel
disease? The Lancet 345 (1995): 1071-1074.
- Wakefield AJ, Puleston JM, Montgomery SM, et al. The
concept of entero‐colonic encephalopathy, autism and
opioid receptor ligands. Alimentary Pharmacology &
Therapeutics 16 (2002): 663-674.
- Wakefield AJ, Ashwood P, ollins, I. The gut-brain axis in
childhood developmental disorders: Viruses and vaccines.
Neuropsychiatric Disorders & Infection (2005): 198.
- Varia J, Herbert M, Hooker B. The neuroimmunology of
autism. (2024).
- Turville C, Golden I. Autism and vaccination: The value
of the evidence base of a recent meta-analysis. Vaccine 33
(2015): 5494-5496.
- Buie T, Campbell DB, Fuchs IIIGJ, et al. Evaluation,
diagnosis, and treatment of gastrointestinal disorders in
individuals with ASD: A consensus report. Pediatrics 125
(2010): S1-S18.
- Casiday RE. Children's health and the social theory of
risk: Insights from the British measles, mumps and
rubella (MMR) controversy. Social Science & Medicine
65 (2007): 1059-1070.
- Shapiro D, Hayburn A. Medical gaslighting as a
mechanism for medical trauma: Case studies and analysis.
Current Psychology (2024): 1-14.
- Williams SJ, Calnan M. The ‘limits’ of medicalization?
Modern medicine and the lay populace in ‘late’ modernity.
Social Science & Medicine 42 (1996): 1609-1620.
- Bennett P, Celik F, Winstanley J, et al. Living with vaccine-
induced immune thrombocytopenia and thrombosis: A
qualitative study. BMJ Open 13 (2023): e072658.
- Fagen JL, Shelton JA, Luché-Thayer J. Medical
gaslighting and Lyme disease: The patient experience. In
Healthcare 12 (2023): 78.
- Leach M. MMR mobilisation: Citizens and science in a
British vaccine controversy. The Institute of Development
Studies & Partner Organizations (2005).
- Angell M. Drug companies & doctors: A story of
corruption. The New York Review of Books 56 (2009):
8-12.
- Mello MM, Abiola S, Colgrove J. Pharmaceutical
companies’ role in state vaccination policymaking: The
case of human papillomavirus vaccination. American
Journal of Public Health 102 (2012): 893-898.
- Lewis DL. Apparent egregious ethical misconduct
by British Medical Journal, Brian Deer. UK Research
Integrity Office (UKRIO), Reference (2012): (2011-060).
- Sharav V. L’Affaire Wakefield: Shades of Dreyfus and
BMJ’s descent into tabloid science. Alliance for Human
Research Protection. (2017).
- Mohammed SA, Rajashekar S, Ravindran SG, et al. Does
vaccination increase the risk of autism spectrum disorder?
Cureus, 14 (2022).
- Turville C, Golden I. Autism and vaccination: The value
of the evidence base of a recent meta-analysis. Vaccine 33
(2015): 5494-5496.
- Poltorak M, Leach M, Fairhead J, et al. ‘MMR talk’ and
vaccination choices: An ethnographic study in Brighton.
Social Science & Medicine 61 (2005): 709-719.
- Hviid A, Hansen JV, Frisch M, et al. Measles, mumps,
rubella vaccination and autism: a nationwide cohort
study. Annals of Internal Medicine 170 (2019): 513-520.
- Jenco MAAP News. Study: MMR vaccine not linked to
increased autism risk.
- Bracho-Sanchez E. CNN. MMR vaccine does not cause
autism, another study confirms. (2019).
- Stein RNPR. A large study provides more evidence that
MMR vaccines don’t cause autism. (2019).
- Geggel L. Live Science. Confirmed: No link between
autism and measles vaccine, even for “at risk” kids.
(2019).
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2135
- Hoffman J. New York Times. One more time, with big
data: Measles vaccine doesn’t cause autism. (2019).
- Mammoser G. Healthline. Another massive study finds
measles vaccine doesn’t cause autism. (2024).
- MedicalNewsToday. Transcripts. MMR vaccine does not
cause autism, even in those most at risk. (2019).
- May B. Psychiatry Advisor. Study again confirms no link
between MMR vaccine and autism. (2019).
- Paumgarten N. The New Yorker. The message of measles.
(2019).
- Madsen KM, Hviid A, Vestergaard M, et al. A population-
based study of measles, mumps, and rubella vaccination
and autism. New England Journal of Medicine 347
(2002): 1477-1482.
- Parner ET, Schendel DE, Thorsen P. Autism prevalence
trends over time in Denmark: Changes in prevalence and
age at diagnosis. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent
Medicine 162 (2008): 1150-1156.
- Kragh Andersen P, Pohar Perme M, van Houwelingen
HC, et al. Analysis of time‐to‐event for observational
studies: Guidance to the use of intensity models. Statistics
in Medicine 40 (2021): 185-211.
- Benkeser D, Carone M, Gilbert PB. Improved estimation
of the cumulative incidence of rare outcomes. Statistics in
Medicine 37 (2018): 280-293.
- Abd ElHafeez S, D’Arrigo G, Leonardis D, et al. Methods
to analyze time‐to‐event data: The cox regression analysis.
Oxidative Medicine & Cellular Longevity 2021 (2021):
1302811.
- Jain A, Marshall J, Buikema A, et al. Autism occurrence
by MMR vaccine status among US children with older
siblings with and without autism. JAMA 313 (2015):
1534-1540.
- Betancur C. Etiological heterogeneity in autism spectrum
disorders: More than 100 genetic and genomic disorders
and still counting. Brain Research 1380 (2011): 42-77.
- Hallmayer J, Cleveland S, Torres A, et al. Genetic
heritability and shared environmental factors among twin
pairs with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry 68
(2011): 1095-1102.
- Gaugler T, Klei L, Sanders SJ, et al. Most genetic risk for
autism resides with common variation. Nature Genetics
46 (2014): 881-885.
- Hocking MC, Albee MV, Kim M, et al. Social challenges,
autism spectrum disorder, and attention deficit/
hyperactivity disorder in youth with neurofibromatosis
type I. Applied Neuropsychology: Child (2024): 1-9.
- Wiznitzer M. Autism and tuberous sclerosis. Journal of
Child Neurology 19 (2004): 675-679.
- Peters SU, Beaudet AL, Madduri N, et al. Autism in
Angelman syndrome: Implications for autism research.
Clinical Genetics 66 (2004): 530-536.
- Abbeduto L, McDuffie A, Thurman AJ. The fragile X
syndrome–autism comorbidity: What do we really know?
Frontiers in Genetics 5 (2014): 355.
- Bennett JA, Germani T, Haqq AM, et al. Autism spectrum
disorder in Prader–Willi syndrome: A systematic review.
American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A 167 (2015):
2936-2944.
- Reilly C. Autism spectrum disorders in Down syndrome:
A review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 3
(2009): 829-839.
- Vorstman JA, Morcus ME, Duijff SN, et al. The 22q11.
2 deletion in children: high rate of autistic disorders
and early onset of psychotic symptoms. Journal of the
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 45
(2006): 1104-1113.
- Bjørklund G, Mkhitaryan M, Sahakyan E, et al. Linking
environmental chemicals to neuroinflammation and
autism spectrum disorder: Mechanisms and implications
for prevention. Molecular Neurobiology (2024): 1-13.
- Park SK, Tao Y, Meeker JD, et al. Environmental
risk score as a new tool to examine multi-pollutants in
epidemiologic research: an example from the NHANES
study using serum lipid levels. PloS One 9 (2014): e98632.
- Liu J, Lewis G. Environmental toxicity and poor cognitive
outcomes in children and adults. Journal of Environmental
Health 76 (2014): 130.
- Maitre L, Bustamante M, Hernández-Ferrer C, et al.
Multi-omics signatures of the human early life exposome.
Nature Communications 13 (2022): 7024.
- Palmer RF, Kattari D, Rincon R, et al. Assessing
chemical intolerance in parents predicts the risk of autism
and ADHD in their children. Journal of Xenobiotics 14
(2024): 350-367.
- Merriam-Webster. Definition of the verb “lie”, meaning
number two. (2024).
- Glickman G, Harrison E, Dobkins K. Vaccination rates
among younger siblings of children with autism. New
England Journal of Medicine 377 (2017): 1099-1101.
- Zerbo O, Modaressi S, Goddard K, et al. Vaccination
patterns in children after autism spectrum disorder
diagnosis and in their younger siblings. JAMA Pediatrics
172 (2018): 469-475.
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2136
- Kuwaik GA, Roberts W, Zwaigenbaum L, et al.
Immunization uptake in younger siblings of children
with autism spectrum disorder. Autism 18 (2014): 148-
155.
- Jain A, Marshall J, Buikema A, et al. Autism occurrence
by MMR vaccine status among US children with older
siblings with and without autism. JAMA 313 (2015):
1534-1540.
- European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control
(ECDC). ECDC vaccine scheduler. Denmark:
Recommended vaccinations. (2024).
- Statens Serum Institut. Danish childhood vaccination
program vaccination offers protection. 13th ed.
Sundhedsstyrelsen. (2024).
- Ingels H, Rasmussen J, Andersen PH, et al. Impact
of pneumococcal vaccination in Denmark during the
first 3 years after PCV introduction in the childhood
immunization programme. Vaccine 30 (2012): 3944-
3950.
- Harboe ZB, Dalby T, Weinberger DM, et al. Impact
of 13-valent pneumococcal conjugate vaccination in
invasive pneumococcal disease incidence and mortality.
Clinical Infectious Diseases 59 (2014): 1066-1073.
- Wójcik OP, Simonsen J, Mølbak K, et al. Validation
of the 5-year tetanus, diphtheria, pertussis and polio
booster vaccination in the Danish childhood vaccination
database. Vaccine 31 (2013): 955-959.
- Harder KM, Cowan S, Eriksen MB, et al. Universal
screening for hepatitis B among pregnant women led to
96% vaccination coverage among newborns of HBsAg
positive mothers in Denmark. Vaccine 29 (2011): 9303-
9307.
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC).
CDC Vaccines & immunizations. Recommended
immunization schedule for persons age 0 through 18
years. (2013).
- US Food and Drug Administration (FDA). (2023).
- World Health Organization (WHO). WHO International.
Human papillomavirus (HPV). (2018).
- Statens Serum Institut. Vaccination against cervical
cancer (HPV). (2018).
- Turville C, Golden I. Autism and vaccination: The value
of the evidence base of a recent meta-analysis. Vaccine
33 (2015): 5494-5496.
- Sorup S, Jensen AK, Aaby P, et al. Revaccination with
measles-mumps-rubella vaccine and infectious disease
morbidity: A Danish register-based cohort study.
Clinical Infectious Diseases 68 (2019): 282-290.
- Sorup S, Jensen AK, Aaby P, et al. Revaccination with
measles-mumps-rubella vaccine and infectious disease
morbidity: A Danish register-based cohort study.
Clinical Infectious Diseases 68 (2019): 282-290.
- Davidkin I, Peltola H, Leinikki P, et al. Duration of
rubella immunity induced by two-dose measles, mumps
and rubella (MMR) vaccination. A 15-year follow-up in
Finland. Vaccine 18 (2000): 3106-3112.
- Paunio M, Heinonen OP, Virtanen M, et al. Measles
history and atopic diseases: A population-based cross-
sectional study. JAMA 283 (2000): 343-346.
- US Food and Drug Administration (FDA). U.S. Food
& Drug Administration (FDA). GlaxoSmithKline
Biologicals SA, Priorix Package Insert. (2022).
- Sorup S, Jensen AK, Aaby P, et al. Revaccination with
measles-mumps-rubella vaccine and infectious disease
morbidity: A Danish register-based cohort study.
Clinical Infectious Diseases 68 (2019): 282-290.
- Gillet Y, Habermehl P, Thomas S, et al. Immunogenicity
and safety of concomitant administration of a measles,
mumps and rubella vaccine (MM-RvaxPro®) and a
varicella vaccine (VARIVAX®) by intramuscular
or subcutaneous routes at separate injection sites: A
randomised clinical trial. BMC Medicine 7 (2009): 1-11.
- European Commission. Sanofi Pasteur, M-M-
RVAXPRO Package Insert. (2024).
- Statens Serum Institut. Uge 24 – 2013. (2024).
- Benn CS. (MD, PhD) Chair of Health Sciences,
Department of Clinical Research, Danish Institute
for Advanced Study (DIAS) University of Southern
Denmark (2024).
- Statens Serum Institut. Pneumokokvaccine I Det Danske
Børnevaccinationsprogram. (2008).
- Parner ET, Schendel DE, Thorsen P. Autism prevalence
trends over time in Denmark: Changes in prevalence and
age at diagnosis. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent
Medicine 162 (2008): 1150-1156.
- Statens Serum Institut. MMR 2 Vaccination Advanced
to 4 Years. (2008).
- Knuesel I, Chicha L, Britschgi M, et al. Maternal
immune activation and abnormal brain development
across CNS disorders. Nature Reviews Neurology 10
(2014): 643-660.
- Wu WL, Hsiao EY, Yan Z, et al. The placental
interleukin-6 signaling controls fetal brain development
and behavior. Brain, Behavior, & Immunity 62 (2017):
11-23.
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2137
- Zerbo O, Qian Y, Yoshida C, et al. Association between
influenza infection and vaccination during pregnancy
and risk of autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Pediatrics
171 (2017): e163609-e163609.
- Hooker BS. Influenza vaccination in the first trimester of
pregnancy and risk of autism spectrum disorder. JAMA
Pediatrics 171 (2017): 600-600.
- Jorgensen P, Mereckiene J, Cotter S, et al. How close
are countries of the WHO European Region to achieving
the goal of vaccinating 75% of key risk groups against
influenza? Results from national surveys on seasonal
influenza vaccination programmes, 2008/2009 to
2014/2015. Vaccine 36 (2018): 442-452.
- Molgaard‐Nielsen D, Fischer TK, Krause TG, et al.
Effectiveness of maternal immunization with trivalent
inactivated influenza vaccine in pregnant women and
their infants. Journal of Internal Medicine 286 (2019):
469-480.
- Nakken CS, Skovdal M, Nellums LB, et al. Vaccination
status and needs of asylum-seeking children in Denmark:
A retrospective data analysis. Public Health 158 (2018):
110-116.
- Cherri Z, Lau K, Nellums LB, et al. The immune status
of migrant populations in Europe and implications
for vaccine-preventable disease control: A systematic
review and meta-analysis. Journal of Travel Medicine,
taae033. (2024).
- Holt N, Mygind A, Bro F. Danish MMR vaccination
coverage is considerably higher than reported. Danish
Medical Journal 64 (2017): A5345.
- Schendel DE, Thorsteinsson E. Cumulative incidence
of autism into adulthood for birth cohorts in Denmark,
1980-2012. JAMA 320 (2018): 1811-1813.
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC).
About Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Data and
Statistics on Autism Spectrum Disorder. (2024).
- Munafò MR, Nosek BA, Bishop DV, et al. A manifesto
for reproducible science. Nature Human Behavior 1
(2017): 1-9.
- Clarkson E (PhD), Chief Statistician at the National
Institute for Aviation Research (NIAR) and Senior
Research Engineer and Chief Statistician for the National
Center for Advanced Materials Performance (NCAMP).
Wichita State University (2019).
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). CDC
Vaccines & Immunizations. 2023. Child and Adolescent
Immunization Schedule by Age. (2023).
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). CDC
Pregnancy and Vaccination. Vaccine recommendations
before, during, and after pregnancy. (2024).
- Bennett J. Shortcomings in the latest MMR vaccination
and autism study: A healthcare administrator’s response.
Science, Public Health Policy & the Law 1 (2019):
2019-2024.
- BioSpace. Biologics market size to hit around USD 1.37
trillion by 2033. (2024).
- Precedence Research. Vaccines market size, share, and
trends 2024 to 2034. (2024).
- Allied Market Research. Mumps vaccine market
size, share, competitive landscape and trend analysis
report, by age group, by distribution channel: Global
opportunity analysis and Industry Forecast 2021-2031.
(2022).
- Parker EM, Zhu BP, Li Z, et al. Domains of excellence:
A CDC framework for developing high-quality, impact-
driven public health science publications. Journal of
Public Health Management & Practice 30 (2024): 72-78.
- Kern JK, Geier DA, Deth RC, et al. Systematic
assessment of research on autism spectrum disorder
(ASD) and mercury reveals conflicts of interest and the
need for transparency in autism research. Science &
Engineering Ethics 23 (2017): 1691-1718.
- Lenzer J. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention:
Protecting the private good? BMJ 350 (2015).
- Huntoon LR. CDC: Bias and disturbing conflicts of
interest. Journal of American Physicians & Surgeons 23
(2020): 66-69.
- Miller NZ, Goldman GS. Case study: Varicella vaccine
and the suppression of data. Journal of American
Physicians and Surgeons 27 (2022).
- Hooker BS. Reanalysis of CDC data on autism incidence
and time of first MMR vaccination. Journal of American
Physicians & Surgeons 23 (2018): 105-110.
- Hooker BS. CDC data manipulation exposed: Four years
later. Journal of American Physicians & Surgeons 22
(2017): 119-121.
- Nissen SE. Conflicts of interest and professional medical
associations: Progress and remaining challenges. JAMA
317 (2017): 1737-1738.
- Deirdre I. Fox News. Conflict of children’s interest
inside the American academy of pediatrics. (2010).
- Steinbrook R, Kassirer JP, Angell M. Justifying conflicts
of interest in medical journals: A very bad idea. BMJ
350 (2015).
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2138
- Angell M, Relman A. S. New York Times. Physicians,
educate yourselves. (2004).
- Angell M. Drug companies & doctors: A story of
corruption. The New York Review of Books 56 (2009):
8-12.
- Statens Serum Institut. Vaccine research. (2022).
- Statens Serum Institut. No Association between MMR
vaccine and autism. (2019).
- Novo Nordisk Foundation. Who are we? (2024) .
- International Committee of Medical Journal Editors.
ICMJE Form for disclosure of potential conflicts of
interest. (2019).
- Indenrigs-OG Sundhedsministeriet. Minister for the
interior and health of Denmark. (2022).
- Novo Nordisk Foundation. Ownership. (2022).
- Novo Nordisk A/S. Corporate governance. (2024).
- CompaniesMarketCap. Global ranking. Market
capitalization of Novo Nordisk (NVO). (2024).
- Novo Holdings. Corporate governance. (2024).
- Novo Holdings. Investments. (2021).
- Novo Holdings. Joining the fight against COVID-19
while running a life-saving business. (2021).
- Novo Nordisk Foundation Initiative for Vaccines and
Immunity (NIVI). Projekter og initiativer. (2023).
- University of Copenhagen. New initiative puts Danish
vaccine science on the global map. (2023).
- NIVI Novo Nordisk Foundation Initiative for Vaccines
and Immunity. About the initiative. (2024).
- Novo Nordisk Foundation. The Novo Nordisk
Foundation Vaccine Accelerator (NVAC). (2024).
- Statens Serum Institut. Vaccination against cervical
cancer (HPV). (2018).
- Olsen SF, Halldorsson TI, Thorne-Lyman AL, et al.
Plasma concentrations of long chain N-3 fatty acids in
early and mid-pregnancy and risk of early preterm birth.
EBioMedicine 35 (2018): 325-333.
- Novo Nordisk Foundation. The Novo Nordisk
Foundation awards grants to 12 excellent research
leaders within bioscience and basic biomedicine. (2019).
- Statens Serum Institut. New research sheds light on
genetics of placenta growth. (2023).
- Statens Serum Institut. SSI researcher receives multi-
million kroner grant from the Novo Nordisk Foundation.
(2024).
- Statens Serum Institut. Babies’ own genes influence
when they are born. (2024).
- Statens Serum Institut. The Danish national biobank.
(2023).
- Angell M, Relman AS. Patents, profits & American
medicine: Conflicts of interest in the testing & marketing
of new drugs. Daedalus 131 (2002): 102-111.
180 Kennedy Jr RF. Thimerosal: Let the science speak:
The evidence supporting the immediate removal of
mercury—A known neurotoxin-From vaccines. Simon
& Schuster. (2015).
- Schuemie MJ, Hripcsak G, Ryan PB, et al. Empirical
confidence interval calibration for population-level
effect estimation studies in observational healthcare
data. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
115 (2018): 2571-2577.
- Hviid A, Stellfeld M, Wohlfahrt J, et al. Association
between thimerosal-containing vaccine and autism.
JAMA 290 (2003): 1763-1766.
- Hviid A, Stellfeld M, Wohlfahrt J, et al. Childhood
vaccination and type 1 diabetes. New England Journal
of Medicine, 350 (2004): 1398-1404.
- Vestergaard M, Hviid A, Madsen KM, et al. MMR
vaccination and febrile seizures: Evaluation of
susceptible subgroups and long-term prognosis. JAMA
292 (2004): 351-357.
- Hviid A, Melbye M, Pasternak B. Use of selective
serotonin reuptake inhibitors during pregnancy and
risk of autism. New England Journal of Medicine 369
(2013): 2406-2415.
- Pasternak B, Svanström H, Hviid A. Ondansetron in
pregnancy and risk of adverse fetal outcomes. New
England Journal of Medicine 368 (2013): 814-823.
- Geier DA, King PG, Hooker BS, et al. Thimerosal:
Clinical, epidemiologic and biochemical studies. Clinica
Chimica acta 444 (2015): 212-220.
- Kern J, Geier D, Audhya T, et al. Evidence of parallels
between mercury intoxication and the brain pathology in
autism. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis 72 (2012):
113-153.
- Mezzacappa A, Lasica PA, Gianfagna F, et al. Risk
for autism spectrum disorders according to period of
prenatal antidepressant exposure: A systematic review
and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatrics 171 (2017): 555-
563.
- Kennedy D. Ondansetron and pregnancy: Understanding
the data. Obstetric Medicine 9 (2016): 28-33.
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2139
- Kirby D. The vaccine-autism court document every
American should read. Huffington Post. (2008).
- Transcripts. Unraveling the mystery of autism; talking
with the CDC director; stories of children with autism;
aging with autism. (2008).
- Reuters. Reuters. Former CDC head lands vaccine job
at Merck. (2009).
- Merck. Dr. Julie L. Gerberding to Retire from Merck.
(2022).
- Hallmayer J, Cleveland S, Torres A, et al. Genetic
heritability and shared environmental factors among
twin pairs with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry
68 (2011): 1095-1102.
- Bradstreet J. Biological evidence of significant vaccine
related side-effects resulting in neurodevelopmental
disorders. Presentation to the Vaccine Safety Committee
of the Institute of Medicine, The National Academies of
Science 9 (2004)..
- Maitre L, Bustamante M, Hernández-Ferrer C, et
- Multi-omics signatures of the human early life
exposome. Nature Communications 13 (2022): 7024.
- Laugesen K, Mengel-From J, Christensen K, et al. A
review of major Danish biobanks: Advantages and
possibilities of health research in Denmark. Clinical
Epidemiology (2023): 213-239.
- Attkisson S. CDC: “Possibility” that vaccines rarely
trigger autism. (2018).
- Price CS, Thompson WW, Goodson B, et al. Prenatal
and infant exposure to thimerosal from vaccines and
immunoglobulins and risk of autism. Pediatrics 126
(2010): 656-664.
- Janiaud P, Agarwal A, Tzoulaki I, et al. Validity of
observational evidence on putative risk and protective
factors: appraisal of 3744 meta-analyses on 57 topics.
BMC Medicine 19 (2021): 1-17.
- Ioannidis JP. Why most clinical research is not useful.
PLoS medicine 13 (2016): e1002049.
- Kennedy RF, Hooker B. Vax-Unvax: Let the science
speak. Skyhorse Publishing. (2023).
- Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, et al. Cochrane handbook
for systematic reviews of interventions. Hoboken, Wiley
(2019).
- Barnes HJ, Ragnarsson G, Alván G. Quality and safety
considerations for recombinant biological medicines: A
regulatory perspective. International Journal of Risk &
Safety in Medicine 21 (2009): 13-22.
- Valiant WG, Cai K, Vallone PM. A history of adventitious
agent contamination and the current methods to detect
and remove them from pharmaceutical products.
Biologicals 80 (2022): 6-17.
- Chooi WH, Ng PW, Hussain Z, et al. Vaccine
contamination: Causes and control. Vaccine 40 (2022):
1699.
- Aranha H, Solutions CP. Virus safety of
biopharmaceuticals. Contract Pharma 13 (2011).
- English JM. The rights and wrongs of measles
vaccination. British Homoeopathic Journal 84 (1995):
156-163.
- Price D, Jefferson T, Demicheli V. Methodological
issues arising from systematic reviews of the evidence
of safety of vaccines. Vaccine 22 (2004): 2080-2084.
- Halvorsen R. The Truth About Vaccines: How we are
used as guinea pigs without knowing it. Gibson Square.
(2007).
- McKinlay JB, McKinlay SM. The questionable
contribution of medical measures to the decline of
mortality in the United States in the twentieth century.
The Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly. Health &
Society (1977): 405-428.
- Armstrong GL, Conn LA, Pinner RW. Trends in
infectious disease mortality in the United States during
the 20th century. JAMA 281 (1999): 61-66.
- Beck MA. The role of nutrition in viral disease. The
Journal of Nutritional Biochemistry 7 (1996): 683-690.
- Hooker BS, Miller NZ. Analysis of health outcomes in
vaccinated and unvaccinated children: Developmental
delays, asthma, ear infections and gastrointestinal
disorders. SAGE Open Medicine 8 (2020):
2050312120925344.
- Miller NZ, Goldman GS. Neonatal, infant, and under age
five vaccine doses routinely given in developed nations
and their association with mortality rates. Cureus 15
(2023).
- Light DW, Lexchin J, Darrow JJ. Institutional corruption
of pharmaceuticals and the myth of safe and effective
drugs. Journal of Law, Medicine & Ethics 41 (2013):
590-600.
- Barosi G, Gale RP. Is there expert consensus on expert
consensus? Bone Marrow Transplantation 53 (2018):
1055-1060.
- Djulbegovic B, Guyatt G. Evidence vs consensus in
clinical practice guidelines. JAMA 322 (2019): 725-726.
- Bragazzi NL, Watad A, Amital H, et al. Debate on
vaccines and autoimmunity: Do not attack the author,
Hammond JR, et al., J Biotechnol Biomed 2025
DOI:10.26502/jbb.2642-91280185
Citation: Jeremy R Hammond, Jeet Varia, Brian Hooker. Hviid et al. 2019 Vaccine-Autism Study: Much Ado About Nothing?. Journal of
Biotechnology and Biomedicine. 8 (2025): 118-140.
Volume 8 • Issue 2140
yet discuss it methodologically. Vaccine 35 (2017):
5522-5526.
- Martin B. On the suppression of vaccination dissent.
Science & Engineering Ethics 21 (2015): 143-157.
- Elisha E, Guetzkow J, Shir-Raz Y, et al. Suppressing
scientific discourse on vaccines? Self-perceptions of
researchers and practitioners. In Hec Forum 36 (2024):
71-89.
- Kempner J. The chilling effect: how do researchers react
to controversy? PLoS Medicine 5 (2008): e222.
- Martin B. The scientific straightjacket: The power
structure of science and the suppression of environmental
scholarship. Ecologist 11 (1981): 33-43.
- Larsson A, Oxman AD, Carling C, et al. Medical
messages in the media–barriers and solutions to
improving medical journalism. Health Expectations 6
(2003): 323-331.
- Shuchman M, Wilkes MS. Medical scientists and health
news reporting: A case of miscommunication. Annals of
Internal Medicine 126 (1997): 976-982.
- Carroll AE. New York Times. Why it’s still worth
getting a flu shot. (2018).
- Carroll AE New York Times. A link between alcohol
and cancer? It’s not nearly as scary as it seems. (2017).
- Lipworth W, Kerridge I, Morrell B, et al. Medicine, the
media and political interests. Journal of Medical Ethics
38 (2012): 768-770.
- Wiley KE, Leask J, Attwell K, et al. Stigmatized for
standing up for my child: A qualitative study of non-
vaccinating parents in Australia. SSM-population Health
16 (2021): 100926.
- Kennedy Jr, RF. Profiles of the vaccine-injured: "A
lifetime price to pay." Simon & Schuster. (2022).
This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the
Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY) license 4.0
That is vile study; not vial study.
Vile makes me sick at my stomach this morning.
Posted by: Benedetta | May 11, 2025 at 06:18 AM
Cassandra, Somewhere South of England :
Brian Hooker along with other fellow scientists have taken all the studies and articles that said MMR does not cause autism and pointed out each of the flaws.
As a matter of fact at the very beginning Brian Hooker said that they really were not even flaws but done in such ways to hide the truth. It was on purpose.
In Brian Hooker's words,"
Hviid et al. did not
faithfully intend or interpret the data to test this hypothesis and, therefore,
cannot possibly have falsified it. We elucidate methodological flaws,
Then ever vial study that we had to endure since the 90s was mentioned.
It is not disjointed, you and I are exhausted, we no longer want to prove what we know as right, but want people drug through the streets to famous prisons. Save "The Rock" for just these criminals.
Posted by: Benedetta | May 11, 2025 at 06:17 AM
Keep the Wealthy Healthy....
Do they have their own private label food brand?
https://cdn2.hubspot.net/hubfs/2685100/pdf/DMPL_PressRelease_on_FieldFresh.pdf
I have always wondered where/how they sourced their food.
Posted by: Emmaphiladelphia | May 10, 2025 at 10:36 PM
Angus
Matt Foley is my personal choice.....
https://www.nbc.com/nbc-insider/chris-farley-matt-foley-van-down-by-the-river-snl-origin
Matt represents MAHA- make America heavy again. With Trump tariffs, we'll all be living in a van down by the river. LOL! He also got the kids off weed- the doorway to psychedelics.
Posted by: Emmaphiladelphia | May 10, 2025 at 09:22 PM
Emmaphiladelphia Spencer Smith has us all doomed on here for sure,mainly for taking responsibility for our own health..The man needs a red pill from Dr Means and keep of the blue pills not good for ya!Spencer.
Pharma For Prison
MMR RIP
Posted by: Angus Files | May 10, 2025 at 05:44 PM
Witchy Woman?
Surgeon General pick has ties to New Age/Occult practices.
Promotes psychedelics?
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=SvS1mHTvqyw
Witchy Woman- The Eagles
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=fCcEjZQQNjo
Posted by: Emmaphiladelphia | May 10, 2025 at 12:24 PM
Casey means appears to support :
"An emerging area: There is research looking into whether psychedelic-assisted therapy can impact the processing of traumatic events and stress and have downstream effects on metabolic health. There is also some evidence that compounds found in psychedelic plants may impact insulin secretion."
https://www.caseymeans.com/learn/article-how-the-mind-controls-metabolism
I think RFK Jr. said his son tried it for depression after his mother died.
I DO NOT support this. I am old enough to remember the '70's hippie drug era.
Posted by: Emmaphiladelphia | May 10, 2025 at 12:09 AM
From the Shannon Joy show notes:
The Truth About TrueMed, Conflicts of interest within MAHA: https://drbowden.substack.com/p/the-truth-about-truemed?r=fuu7w&utm_medium=ios&triedRedirect=true
Embalmers are Continuing to Find Mysterious Clots in the Vaccinated: https://www.midwesterndoctor.com/p/embalmers-are-continuing-to-find?utm_source=substack&utm_campaign=post_embed&utm_medium=web
Greg Harrison and Tom Haviland on white fibrous clots: https://x.com/SenseReceptor/status/1909741957942771987
Also, just out from CHD publishing:
The Ultimate Vaccine Timeline
A Fact-Packed History of Vaccines and Their Makers
Shaz Khan, Pierre Kory
https://www.skyhorsepublishing.com/9781648210679/the-ultimate-vaccine-timeline/
This looks useful and interesting.
Posted by: Emmaphiladelphia | May 09, 2025 at 05:17 PM
My, how things changed under Biden.....
"The immigration law of 1891 made it mandatory that all immigrants coming into the United States be given a health inspection by the Public Health Service physicians. The law stipulated the exclusion of "all idiots, insane persons, paupers or persons likely to become public charges, persons suffering from a loathsome or dangerous contagious disease," and criminals. The largest inspection center was on Ellis Island in New York Harbor."
The origins of vaccines and the U.S. Public Health service was the slave trade/plantation economic system before 1865. That inconvenient fact was left out of the Public Health Service history, which began with the establishment of marine hospitals at major slave trade points of entry:
"John Adams, second President of the United States, signed into law on July 16, 1798 the Act for the Relief of Sick and Disabled Seamen, which established what is now the Public Health Service. Twenty cents was deducted from the monthly wage of each merchant seaman to build or rent hospitals and pay for the medical care provided."
https://www.nlm.nih.gov/exhibition/phs_history/seamen.html
Carefully examine this Public Health Service timeline and recognize that until 1865, the communicable diseases the seamen were being treated for had its origins in the horrific conditions on the ships of the African triangle trade. This is graphically illustrated by the account of U.S. Senator and slave trader James DeWolf:
"In 1791, a warrant was issued for the arrest of James DeWolf. He was captain of the Polly, a slave ship, sailing from Africa to Cuba with 142 slaves and 15 crew. When an enslaved African woman, middle-aged, came down with smallpox, James DeWolf ordered her quarantined. She was tied to a chair and brought above deck. When she got worse, DeWolf asked for a volunteer to throw the sick woman overboard. The crew refused.
James DeWolf decided to do it himself. He blindfolded and gagged her so the other slaves couldn’t hear her scream. Then he asked a sailor to help him with a grappling hook, which they attached to her chair. The two men lowered her into the ocean. She sank immediately, drowned and died.
According to a crewman’s testimony, James DeWolf said he regretted the loss of such a good chair.
What James DeWolf did wasn’t unusual. Sick slaves were often tossed overboard. But murder on the high seas was illegal under the Federal Crimes Act of 1790. In 1791, a warrant was issued for the arrest of James DeWolf.
What James DeWolf did wasn’t unusual. Sick slaves were often tossed overboard. But murder on the high seas was illegal under the Federal Crimes Act of 1790."
https://newenglandhistoricalsociety.com/james-dewolf-ri-senator-murdered-slave-got-away/
Jenner's vaccine was also introduced into America during the era of slave trade:
"Dr. Benjamin Waterhouse (1754-1846) introduced into the United States in 1800 the technique of smallpox vaccination discovered in England by Dr. Edward Jenner. Smallpox was one of the most dreaded epidemic diseases in America during the 17th and 18th centuries."
https://www.nlm.nih.gov/exhibition/phs_history/seamen.html
Slave traders and Southern plantation owners would have been some of his biggest customers. I am sure the slave populations provided ample "guinea pigs" for vaccine testing.
Posted by: Emmaphiladelphia | May 09, 2025 at 02:52 PM
MAHA CIVIL WAR! Why MAHA Is Fighting Over Dr Casey Means As U.S. Surgeon General
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=9Nyzxs3HZEU
Kim Iverson does a great job breaking this down.
Vax/food bait and switch?
Posted by: Emmaphiladelphia | May 09, 2025 at 03:23 AM
I’m finding this article very difficult to read because of the disjointed display. Is there any way that this can be altered? Thank you in advance.
Posted by: Cassandra, Somewhere South of England | May 09, 2025 at 02:31 AM
APR 15, 2025 BREAKING — Autism Prevalence in U.S. Kids Jumps 16.1% in Just Two Years
New CDC data reveals 1 in 31 (3.22%) of American 8-year-olds were autistic in 2022 — a staggering 384% increase since 2000.
https://www.thefocalpoints.com/p/breaking-autism-prevalence-in-us?utm_source=post-email-title&publication_id=1119676&post_id=161409446&utm_campaign=email-post-title&isFreemail=true&r=1ifz5&triedRedirect=true&utm_medium=email
Posted by: Gary Brown | May 08, 2025 at 09:21 PM
Meet Trump's New Surgeon General
https://rumble.com/v6t4vyd-meet-trumps-new-surgeon-general.html
Posted by: Emmaphiladelphia | May 08, 2025 at 09:02 PM
CLEANUP ON AISLE FDA! Send in the clown distraction!
https://servicetoamericamedals.org/honorees/peter-marks/
Posted by: Emmaphiladelphia | May 08, 2025 at 06:46 PM
Let's not forget the Covid 19 EUA jab/ Comirnaty "bait and switch."
MEDIA CALL: First COVID-19 Vaccine Approval - 8/23/2021
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=aHAGnDz9F_w&t=958s
Peter Marks and Janet Woodcock work their verbal gymnastics!
Comirnaty was not available in the US in the Covid era- only the unlicensed experimental EUA version. That version didn't have to have uniform ingredient contents. Is that why certain batches had all the adverse events?
The Highwire exposes Dr. Marks:
ICAN RELEASES ‘SECRET RECORDINGS: THE REAL PETER MARKS’ TIMELINE
https://thehighwire.com/ark-videos/ican-releases-secret-recordings-the-real-peter-marks-timeline/
Posted by: Emmaphiladelphia | May 08, 2025 at 06:03 PM
Passed these on to my pro-vaxxer senator:
https://cdn.fortunejournals.com/articles/hviid-et-al-2019-vaccineautism-study-much-ado-about-nothing.pdf
ICAN RELEASES ‘SECRET RECORDINGS: THE REAL PETER MARKS’ TIMELINE
https://thehighwire.com/ark-videos/ican-releases-secret-recordings-the-real-peter-marks-timeline/
https://therealpetermarks.com/
Posted by: Emmaphiladelphia | May 08, 2025 at 03:44 PM